FIGURE 4–4 Generalizability theory model with two facets— raters and item type.

treated as a random facet, it would be considered a random sample of all possible tasks generated under the same rules to measure the construct, and the results of the g-study would be considered to generalize to that universe of tasks. When facets are treated as fixed, the results are considered to generalize only to the elements of the facet in the study. Using the same example, if the set of tasks were treated as fixed, the results would generalize only to the tasks at hand (see Kreft and De Leeuw, 1998, for a discussion of this and other usages of the terms “random” and “fixed”).

In practice, researchers carry out a g-study to ascertain how different facets affect the reliability (generalizability) of scores. This information can then guide decisions about how to design sound situations for making observations—for example, whether to average across raters, add more tasks, or test on more than one occasion. To illustrate, in the BEAR assessment, a g-study could be carried out to see which type of assessment—embedded tasks or link items—contributed more to reliability. Such a study could also be used to examine whether teachers were as consistent as external raters. Generalizability models offer two powerful practical advantages. First, they allow one to characterize how the conditions under which the observations were made affect the reliability of the evidence. Second, this information is expressed in terms that allow one to project from the current assessment design to other potential designs.



The National Academies | 500 Fifth St. N.W. | Washington, D.C. 20001
Copyright © National Academy of Sciences. All rights reserved.
Terms of Use and Privacy Statement