Bullock, M., Gelman, R., and Baillargeon, R. (1982). The development of causal reasoning. In W.J. Friedman (Ed.), The developmental psychology of time. New York: Academic Press.

Brown, D.E., and Clement, J. (1989). Overcoming misconceptions via analogical reasoning: Abstract transfer versus explanatory model construction. Instructional Science, 18, 237-261.

Bruner, J. (1964). The course of cognitive growth. American Psychologist, 19, 1-15.

Caramazza, A., McCloskey, M., and Green, B. (1981). Naive beliefs in “sophisticated” subjects: Misconceptions about trajectories of objects. Cognition, 9, 117-123.

Carey, S. (1985). Conceptual change in childhood. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Carey, S. (1988). Conceptual differences between children and adults. Mind and Language, 3, 167-181.

Carey, S. (1991). Knowledge acquisition: Enrichment or conceptual change? In S. Carey and R. Gelman (Eds.), The epigenesis of mind: Essays on biology and cognition (pp. 257-291). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Chi, M.T.H. (2005). Commonsense conceptions of emergent processes: Why some misconceptions are robust. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 14(2), 161-199.

Clement, J. (1982). Students’ preconceptions in introductory mechanics. American Journal of Physics, 50, 66-71.

Cohen, L.B., and Cashon, C.H. (2006). Infant cognition. In W. Damon and R.M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, 6th Edition. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Csibra, G., Gergely, G., Biró, S., Koós, O., and Brockbank, M. (1999). Goal-attribution without agency cues: The perception of ‘pure reason’ in infancy. Cognition, 72, 237-267.

DeLoache, J.S. (1987). Rapid change in the symbolic functioning of very young children. Science, 238, 1556-1557.

DeLoache, J.S. (2004). Becoming symbol-minded. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 66-70.

DeLoache, J.S., Pierroutsakos, S.L., and Uttal, D.H. (2003). The origins of pictorial competence. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 114-118.

Dickinson, L. (1987). Self-instruction in language learning. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

diSessa, A.A. (2004). Metarepresentation: Native competence and targets for instruction. Cognition and Instruction, 22(3), 293-331.

Doran, R.L. (1972). Misconceptions of selected science held by elementary school students. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 9, 127-137.

Driver, R., Leach, J., Millar, R., and Scott, P. (1996). Young people’s images of science. Buckingham, England: Open University Press.

Driver, R., Squires, A., Rushworth, P., and Wood-Robinson, V. (1994). Making sense of secondary science: Research into children’s ideas. New York: Routledge.

Duschl, R. (2000). Making the nature of science explicit. In R. Millar, J. Leech, and J. Osborne (Eds.), Improving science education: The contribution of research (pp. 187-206). Philadelphia, PA: Open University Press.

Espinoza, F. (2005). Experience conflicts with and undermines instruction and its legacy: Examples from two-dimensional motion. Physics Education, 40(3), 274-280.



The National Academies | 500 Fifth St. N.W. | Washington, D.C. 20001
Copyright © National Academy of Sciences. All rights reserved.
Terms of Use and Privacy Statement