Vosniadou, S., and Ioannides, C. (1998). From conceptual development to science education: A psychological point of view. International Journal of Science Education, 20(10), 1213-1230.

Watson, J.S. (1979). Perception of contingency as a determinant of social responsiveness. In E.B. Thoman (Ed.), Origins of the infant’s social responsiveness (pp. 33-64). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Wellman, H.M. (1990). The child’s theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Werner, H., and Kaplan, B. (1963). Symbol formation: An organismic developmental approach to language and the expression of thought. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Wimmer, H., and Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representations and constraining functions of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103-128.

Woodward, A.L. (1998). Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor’s reach. Cognition, 69(1), 1-34.

Woodward, A.L., Sommerville, J.A., and Guajardo, J.J. (2001). How infants make sense of intentional action. In B.F. Malle, L.J. Moses, and D.A. Baldwin (Eds.), Intentions and intentionality. Foundations of social cognition (pp. 149-170). Boston, MA: MIT Press.

Yoachim, C.M., and Meltzoff, A.N. (2003, October). Cause and effect in the mind of the preschool child. Poster presented at the biennial meeting of the Cognitive Development Society, Park City, UT.

Zimmer, C. (2004). Soul made flesh. New York: Free Press.



The National Academies | 500 Fifth St. N.W. | Washington, D.C. 20001
Copyright © National Academy of Sciences. All rights reserved.
Terms of Use and Privacy Statement