The National Academies | 500 Fifth St. N.W. | Washington, D.C. 20001
Copyright © National Academy of Sciences. All rights reserved.
Terms of Use and Privacy Statement



Below are the first 10 and last 10 pages of uncorrected machine-read text (when available) of this chapter, followed by the top 30 algorithmically extracted key phrases from the chapter as a whole.
Intended to provide our own search engines and external engines with highly rich, chapter-representative searchable text on the opening pages of each chapter. Because it is UNCORRECTED material, please consider the following text as a useful but insufficient proxy for the authoritative book pages.

Do not use for reproduction, copying, pasting, or reading; exclusively for search engines.

OCR for page 152

OCR for page 152
JOSEPH HAROLD GREENBERG May 28, 1915–May 7, 2001 By WILLIAM CROFT J oseph h. g reenberg, o ne o f t he most original and influen- tial linguists of the 20th century, died at his home in Stanford, California, on May 7, 2001, three weeks before his 86th birthday. Greenberg was a major pioneer in the development of linguistics as an empirical science. He came of intellectual age at a time when linguistics was establishing itself as an independent academic discipline, and helped to shape the field. Greenberg’s work was always founded directly on quantitative data from a single language or from a wide range of languages. His chief legacy to contemporary linguistics lies in the areas of language universals and his- torical linguistics. Greenberg is the founder of the modern typological approach to language universals, in which lan- guage universals are discovered inductively by the examina- tion of a worldwide sample of languages, and explained in terms of the function of language, including the meanings conveyed by grammatical structures and constraints imposed by our abilities to comprehend and produce utterances. The typological approach has been tremendously influen- tial, and is often compared to the generative approach of This is a revised version of an obituary that appeared in Language 77(2001):815-830 and is used by permission of the Linguistic Society of America. 153

OCR for page 152
154 BIOGRAPHICAL MEMOIRS Chomsky. Greenberg’s paper “Some Universals of Grammar with Particular Reference to the Order of Meaningful Ele- ments” (1963) is one of the most cited papers in the field of linguistics. Greenberg also single-handedly reformed the genetic classification of the languages of Africa, and aroused major controversies (which still continue) in his later work in the genetic classification of the languages of Oceania, Eurasia, and the Americas—in other words, the languages of the entire world. Greenberg also shifted his- torical linguistics toward the study of universals of language change, including the study of grammaticalization, one of the most active areas in historical linguistics today. In addition to these seminal and far-reaching contributions, Greenberg also made major contributions to sociolinguistics, psycholinguistics, phonetics and phonology, morphology, and especially African language studies. Joe Greenberg was born on May 28, 1915, in Brooklyn, New york, the second of two children. His father was a Pol- ish Jew, his mother a German Jew. His father’s family name was originally Zyto, but in one of those turn-of-the-century immigrant stories, his father ended up taking the name of his landlord. Joe Greenberg’s early loves were music and languages. As a child he sat fascinated next to his mother while she played the piano, and one day he asked her to teach him. She taught him musical notation and then found him a local teacher. Greenberg ended up studying with a Madame Vangerova, associated with the Curtis Institute of Music. Greenberg even gave a concert at Steinway Hall at the age of 14, and won a citywide prize for best chamber music ensemble. But after finishing high school Greenberg chose an academic career instead of a musical one, although he continued to play the piano every evening until near the end of his life.

OCR for page 152
155 JOSEPH HAROLD GREENBERG Greenberg’s fascination with languages began equally early. He went to Hebrew school, which offered only an el- ementary education in Hebrew, essentially how to read the script. But Greenberg got hold of a Hebrew grammar and taught himself the language. He studied Latin and German at James Madison High School. He had a friend at the Erasmus High School who studied Classical Greek, but James Madison High School didn’t offer Greek. Greenberg learned as much Greek as he could from a parallel-text edition of Sophocles’ plays and the etymologies of the Oxford dictionary, and asked his father if he could transfer to Erasmus High School. They went to see the principal, who asked Joe why he wanted to study Greek, and he simply said, “I’d like to study Greek,” and the principal refused his transfer. On the way home Joe cried, and his father took him into town and bought him a Greek grammar and dictionary from a used bookstore. With those books, he taught himself Greek—in fact, that was the usual way he learned languages. When Greenberg began college at Columbia University in 1932, he continued studying Latin and Greek and also taught himself Classical Arabic. He signed up for classes in obscure languages, such as Akkadian and various Slavic languages, annoying professors who thought they could get away without teaching by offering classes they thought nobody would take. He discovered comparative linguistics in his junior year and anthropology in his senior year. Also in his senior year he audited a class given by Franz Boas on American Indian languages and, on his own, read all the grammars in Boas’s Handbook of American Indian Languages (Boas, 1911, 1922). Because of his Classical and Semitic background, Greenberg entertained the idea of becoming a medieval historian specializing in contacts between Christian- ity and Islam in Africa. But opportunities in the humanities were nonexistent during the Depression. His anthropology

OCR for page 152
156 BIOGRAPHICAL MEMOIRS professor, Alexander Lesser, suggested he apply for a Social Science Research Council Ph.D. grant to study under Melville Herskovits, a major Africanist at Northwestern University. Lesser obtained references for him from Boas and Ruth Benedict. Greenberg received the grant and studied with Herskovits at Northwestern. In his third year Greenberg did fieldwork among the Hausa in Nigeria (learning Hausa in the process), and wrote a dissertation on the influence of Islam on one of the few remaining non-Islamic Hausa groups. Greenberg’s intellectual interests continued to expand. Herskovits encouraged him to spend his second year at yale (1937-1938), where he studied with the anthropologists Leslie Spier and Robert Lowie and the linguists Edgar Sturtevant and Franklin Edgerton. (He never met the linguist and an- thropologist Edward Sapir, always Joe’s hero; Sapir was ill at the time and died in 1940.) The linguistics courses were all on comparative Indo-European. It was not until he returned to yale with a postdoctoral fellowship in 1940 that he made his acquaintance with American structuralism, auditing courses with Bernard Bloch, George L. Trager and Benjamin Lee Whorf, all leading structuralists. Greenberg also met Leonard Bloomfield, considered the founder of American structuralist linguistics, at around this time, though not at yale. Bloomfield suggested to Greenberg that he read the analytical philosopher Rudolf Carnap and thereby intro- duced Greenberg to logical positivism. Greenberg studied Alfred North Whitehead and Bertrand Russell’s Principia Mathematica, even taking it with him when he was drafted into the Army in 1940. Logical positivism had a significant influence on Greenberg, not only in the general rigor of its argumentation; he published axiomatizations of kinship systems and phonology.

OCR for page 152
157 JOSEPH HAROLD GREENBERG Greenberg took the postdoctoral position at yale because there were no academic positions in the Depression, espe- cially for Jews. Being drafted into the Army in 1940 solved the employment problem for five years. Before he left for the war, he married Selma Berkowitz, whom he had met when she was finishing high school and he was starting at Colum- bia; she remained his companion and support for the rest of his life. Greenberg entered the Army Signal Corps and was eventually sent to North Africa, participating in the landing at Casablanca. In North Africa he and his colleagues got up in the middle of the night and deciphered the German or Italian code by the early morning. After the Allied invasion of Italy he was sent to Italy, where he remained until the end of the war—and where he learned Italian, of course. Conditions for academic employment were completely different after World War II. The GI Bill offered funding for GIs to go to college, and universities expanded. This expan- sion continued as the postwar baby boom eventually made its way to college. Greenberg was appointed at the University of Minnesota in 1946 and moved to the anthropology depart- ment at Columbia University in 1948. The leading European structuralists Roman Jakobson and André Martinet, recent arrivals from Europe, had founded the Linguistic Circle of New york. Through them Greenberg was exposed to the structuralism of the Prague school, including Nicholas Trubetzkoy’s work on markedness. (Greenberg also coedited Word, the journal of the Linguistic Circle of New york, from 1950 to 1954.) Greenberg’s intellectual roots included all of the major strands of linguistics, philosophy, and anthropology at the time: American structuralism, Prague-school structuralism, comparative historical linguistics, logical positivism, and cultural anthropology. (This is not to mention his Classical

OCR for page 152
158 BIOGRAPHICAL MEMOIRS and Semitic background, or his awesomely broad reading, which continued to the end of his life.) At the time, the first linguistics departments in America were being estab- lished, and Greenberg was in a position to help shape the field of linguistics. At that time, the field was still largely divided among philologists working on historical linguis- tics and anthropological field linguists working on “exotic” languages. Linguistics, chiefly in the form of structuralism, was still in the process of declaring its academic and intel- lectual independence from philology and anthropology. Greenberg made major contributions to the establishment of linguistics as an independent science. Greenberg’s first major work brought the young Ameri- can linguist immediate fame and controversy. Greenberg’s Ph.D. research was as an Africanist, and he turned his atten- tion to African languages. His first large project was nothing if not ambitious: the genetic classification of the languages of Africa. After a preliminary publication in American An- thropologist in 1948, a complete classification was published in serialized form in the Southwestern Journal of Anthropology in 1949-1950. At the time, African languages were classified into five families: Semitic, Hamitic, Sudanic, Bantu, and Bushman (Newman, 1995, p. 3). Greenberg classified them instead into 16 families, on the basis of two fundamental principles. His first principle is the exclusion of typological fea- tures from genetic classification. That is, properties purely of form (phonological patterns or grammatical patterns) or of meaning (semantic patterns) are too likely to diffuse, are too small in number, and are too likely to result from inde- pendent convergence to act as indicators of genetic descent. Instead, the arbitrary pairings of form and meaning in both morphology and lexicon provide the best evidence for genetic classification. This separation of typological and genetic traits

OCR for page 152
159 JOSEPH HAROLD GREENBERG of languages provided the key to genetic classification, and almost as a by-product produced the independent develop- ment of typology a few years later in Greenberg’s career. His second principle is the exclusion of nonlinguistic evidence from the establishment of linguistic genetic families. Both of these principles were violated by the accepted classifica- tions of African languages. Typological traits, such as the presence or absence of gender, and nonlinguistic factors, such as race, played a role in the accepted classification. Greenberg’s proposals cut across the accepted clas- sification but established the basic principles for genetic classification of languages. As a young American scholar he upset the senior British and German scholars in the field. Greenberg knew he was overthrowing established academic views, as is clear from the preliminary article in American Anthropologist, in which he fearlessly takes on the authori- ties in his field. Greenberg did not stop at African language classifica- tion. He turned to the study of the languages of the Ameri- cas, Australia, and other parts of the world. While North American languages were at the time grouped into a small number of large families, South American languages were not, and so Greenberg began with South America, where he identified seven families. In Australia he identified one wide- spread family, which he called General Australian (identical to Pama-Nyungan), and a large number of small families. Some of these observations were published in “Historical Linguistics and Unwritten Languages” (1953). In response to criticisms of that paper and his other work, Greenberg explicitly formulated his third and final principle of genetic classification, namely the simultaneous comparison of the full range of languages and forms for the area under study (mass comparison, later called multilateral comparison). In

OCR for page 152
160 BIOGRAPHICAL MEMOIRS 1955 Greenberg’s African classification was reprinted, and he had consolidated the 16 families to 12. He continued his classification work, proposing 14 families for the non-Aus- tronesian, non-Australian languages in Oceania, in a report to the Wenner-Gren Foundation in 1958. Greenberg did not fully realize the ramifications of his method until, as he described it later, “One day, probably in early 1959, as I put my foot on the pavement to cross Amsterdam Avenue on my way to Columbia, an idea flashed before me. Why shouldn’t I just look at all of my then twelve families in Africa together?” (Greenberg 1996a, p. 147). He did so. His final classification of African languages (1963) consists of four families: Afroasiatic, Khoisan, Niger-Kor- dofanian, and Nilo-Saharan. This classification has been broadly accepted but not until after a lengthy and heated debate. In this debate many of the British and German Africanists defended their typological and nonlinguistic classifications of African languages. A crucial number of mostly American Africanists accepted Greenberg’s results and the method that he used. Other linguists, in particular Americanists and Indo-Europeanists, argued that only re- construction of the protolanguage would “prove” the genetic classification of languages (see Croft, 2005a). Almost all of these arguments would recur from the late 1980s up to the present, as Greenberg’s classifications in other parts of the world were published. As a matter of fact, Greenberg’s more controversial clas- sifications of the languages of Oceania, the Americas and Eurasia were all arrived at by the early 1960s. Greenberg presented evidence in 1960 that the 14 families he identi- fied in Oceania belong to a single group. In the same year, a paper originally presented in 1956 was published, proposing that the American Indian languages fall into three genetic groupings. (Interestingly, the linguists Sydney Lamb and

OCR for page 152
161 JOSEPH HAROLD GREENBERG Morris Swadesh independently arrived at the same conclu- sion at about the same time.) In the course of his research on the American Indian languages, he compared them with languages in northern Eurasia and by the early 1960s had identified another large family ranging from Indo-European in the west to Eskimo-Aleut in the east. But Greenberg did not publish the evidence for these proposals until many years later, and for this reason I will return to the 1950s to take up the other strands of Greenberg’s contributions to linguistics. Much of Greenberg’s earlier work is on African linguis- tics, and he was recognized early on as one of the leading African language scholars. (In fact, he was an unnerving presence at African language conferences, where he would sit in the front row and ask questions revealing his knowledge of virtually every African language that was the topic of a paper.) In addition to the classification of African languages, he wrote numerous articles on phonology and morphology, particularly in Afroasiatic, and on language contact in Africa. In “The Patterning of Root Morphemes in Semitic” (1950), Greenberg displayed his characteristic thorough, quantitative approach to a linguistic problem. He examined 3775 Arabic triliteral roots and surveyed roots in other Semitic languages in order to formulate a number of constraints on the occur- rence of phonemes and phonological features across the root consonants of Semitic. The article also displays his breadth of knowledge of the literature and citation of antecedents and parallel discovery. It is a pathbreaking work, one that has been repeatedly cited in later research on morpheme structure conditions and phonotactic constraints. Greenberg described himself as being in a state of intellectual ferment, or even crisis, from 1952 to 1954 (Green- berg, 1994, p. 23). Although not formally trained as a linguist,

OCR for page 152
162 BIOGRAPHICAL MEMOIRS he had been influenced by American structuralism and its seeming philosophical counterpart, logical postivism. yet he had recognized some of structuralism’s weaknesses, partly through influences such as the Prague School and compara- tive-historical linguistics (which had much greater prestige then than now). In particular, he questioned American structuralism’s lack of interest in meaning and use, the strict separation of synchrony and diachrony, and the methods for uncovering basic linguistic units such as the phoneme. Greenberg recalled another formative experience that occurred in 1953. He was part of an interdisciplinary semi- nar on linguistics and psychology organized by the Social Science Research Council. He presented the current state of linguistics, that is, the rigorous methodology of Ameri- can structuralism, about which he already had misgivings. The psychologist Charles Osgood told Greenberg that that was quite impressive, but that if Greenberg could tell him something true of all languages, then psychologists would be interested. Greenberg later said that “this remark . . . brought home to me the realization that all of contemporary American linguistics consisted of elaborate but essentially descriptive procedures” and that Osgood’s question “helped determine the direction of much of my future work” (Greenberg, 1986, pp. 13-14). Greenberg then turned to the problem of uni- versals of language. It would be four years, however, before Greenberg published his first paper on language universals. Instead, his next major publication in synchronic theory was in the area of typology (1954,1). At that time typology was the study of language differences, not similarities, based on phonological and morphological traits. The morphological typology of the 19th century, dividing languages into isolating, agglutinative, and inflectional types, was the only major typological classifi-

OCR for page 152
171 JOSEPH HAROLD GREENBERG Greenberg participated vigorously in this debate, con- tributing some 20 responses, replies, commentaries, and reviews of his critics. He consistently maintained his position on his genetic classifications. He argued that a quantitative probabilistic argument is required to “prove” an empirical scientific hypothesis, in response to the historical linguists who argued that reconstruction of the protolanguage was necessary. Greenberg further argued that, in fact, his method necessarily precedes reconstruction, since reconstruction presupposes a classification He noted that he used the same methods for linguistic genetic classification in the Americas as he did in Africa, now generally accepted, and that this method was the same used to identify the now-accepted language families in the 18th and 19th centuries. Finally, he argued that alternative, nongenetic hypotheses for the widespread similarities in form and meaning in words and grammatical elements across languages, such as extensive language mixing, extensive borrowing of basic vocabulary and grammatical inflections, or sound symbolism, were either sociolinguistically implausible or not persuasively supported by attested sociohistorical developments in shallower, widely accepted language families. Another controversial aspect of Greenberg’s Amerind hypothesis was the support it received from physical anthro- pology and from genetics. Stephen Zegura and Christy Turner independently hypothesized a three-migration pattern into the Americas based on dentition and genetic evidence; the three of them published their results together (1986). In addition, the geneticist Luigi Luca Cavalli-Sforza compared genetic groupings of humans and Greenberg’s linguistic classification, and found a high degree of similarity (Cavalli- Sforza et al., 1988; see also Greenberg, 1996b), which led to further controversy. Greenberg was of course encouraged by this convergence of independent evidence, but he always

OCR for page 152
172 BIOGRAPHICAL MEMOIRS insisted that the linguistic classification must be established on linguistic evidence alone. Greenberg continued to publish on diachronic, typo- logical, and other topics, but the main focus of his research after his retirement from Stanford was genetic classification. His next area of study was Eurasia. He continued to gather lexical and grammatical evidence for a family he called Eurasiatic, consisting of Indo-European, Uralic, Altaic, Korean-Japanese-Ainu, Gilyak, Chukchi-Kamchatkan, and Eskimo-Aleut. Although many scholars had compared pairs of families (e.g., Indo-European and Uralic, Uralic and Al- taic, Altaic and Japanese), Greenberg argued that all of the aforementioned groups together constitute a valid genetic unit. He published a number of articles presenting parts of this evidence, and eventually included 72 independent pieces of grammatical evidence in a monograph, Indo-Euro- pean and Its Closest Relatives: The Eurasiatic Language Family, vol. 1, Grammar (2000,1). Although Greenberg did not know at the time that he had only two years to live, at the age of 84 he proceeded to write the second volume (the lexical evidence) at a frantic pace. “I am fighting against time to get the second volume finished,” he wrote me in January 2000. He submitted the final etymologies to Merritt Ruhlen for typing on October 27, 2000, and went into the hospital that evening. He was diagnosed with pancreatic cancer, and stayed home with his wife, Selma, from then until his death. But he worked until mid-March 2001 with Ruhlen on finalizing the lexical evidence. Up to the last month of his life Joe was still incredibly active. Even in my last conversation with him, a week before he died, he could joke that he could have written five papers in the months since he had been diagnosed. Joe didn’t want

OCR for page 152
173 JOSEPH HAROLD GREENBERG to stop. He wanted to pursue the classification of languages all the way up to the human language family, which he thought possible; and of course there were all those fasci- nating processes of language change that he encountered on the way. He planned to turn next to a southern group consisting most likely of Niger-Kordofanian, Nilo-Saharan, Elamo-Dravidian, Indo-Pacific, and Australian. He retrieved his old notebooks but realized that he needed more sources and did not have the time and energy to proceed. Fortunately, Joe also recognized that he had lived as full a scholarly life as one could ask for and that his published work (including the work to appear after his death) would leave a legacy that would extend far into the future. During his long life Greenberg received many acco- lades: twice fellow at the Center for Advanced Study of the Behavioral Sciences, thrice Guggenheim fellow, elected to the American Philosophical Society and the National Academy of Sciences (in 1965), president of the Linguistic Society of America, the African Studies Association, the West African Linguistic Association, and the Linguistic So- ciety of America Collitz Professor. Greenberg gave the first Distinguished Lecture of the American Anthropological As- sociation, and received the Haile Selassie Award for African Research, the New york Academy of Sciences Award in Be- havioral Science, and the American Academy of Arts and Sciences Talcott Parsons Prize in Social Science. Stanford University planned a conference to honor Greenberg, “Global Perspectives on Human Language,” which Joe hoped to take part in, but sadly it became a conference in his memory in April 2002. Despite the controversial positions he took from the be- ginning of his career to the end, and the stature he gained in the field, Joe Greenberg was one of the most mild-mannered

OCR for page 152
174 BIOGRAPHICAL MEMOIRS and self-effacing scholars imaginable. He was unbelievably modest and unassuming for such a brilliant scientist. The reason, I believe, was that he always had a completely genuine curiosity and wonder at language and, indeed, at everything in the world. He also had an unpretentious, down-to-earth way of talking about languages—reinforced by his thick Brooklyn accent, no doubt, and the equally down-to-earth similes he used. He once said, “A speaker is like a lousy auto mechanic: Every time he fixes something in the language, he screws up something else.” Another memorable remark came when Joe revived his typology class in the fall of 1984, while I was still a graduate student at Stanford. One day Joe was describing some interesting fact about a language, and he suddenly stopped and said, “you know, you gotta muck around in grammars. you can’t just focus on one specific thing and pick it out. you read around and you discover things you never would have thought of.” Joe was a completely independent intellectual spirit. He was not so much an iconoclast as someone who considered nothing above questioning or beneath consideration. He absorbed comparative historical linguistics from Bloomfield, Sturtevant, and Edgerton, but did not let its strictures about reconstruction prevent him from pursuing genetic linguistic classification. He learned American structuralism from Bloch, Trager, and Whorf, but did not accept their ban on meaning nor their antiuniversalist stance. He continued his typologi- cal approach to universals, developed at the same time as generative grammar, while the rest of American linguistics fell under Chomsky’s spell. Joe sometimes attributed his independence to the fact that he didn’t study linguistics in a linguistics department. But Joe was deeply knowledgeable about the history of lin- guistics. (I never had the opportunity to take the history of linguistics from him, but he told me that he usually got to

OCR for page 152
175 JOSEPH HAROLD GREENBERG around the Renaissance by the end of the course.) He could quote freely from the great 19th-century German historical linguists; but he also followed developments in contemporary linguistic theories and, of course, read the specialist language journals. In fact, most of Joe’s learning came from reading: logic, philosophy, languages, linguistics, anthropology, his- tory, culture, biology, and so on. Joe lamented to me that students no longer received the broad humanistic education that he did—but he largely gave that education to himself. Joe was the scholar’s scholar. His office was Green Library at Stanford, where he worked all day six days a week, always reading and making notes in pencil in his famous notebooks. The library staff one day surprised him by installing a brass plaque on the oak reading table where he worked, inscribed “The Joseph H. Greenberg Research Table.” Joe’s erudition was awesome, but he wore it lightly. He could recall obscure facts about languages anywhere in the world, though in later years he said, “Every time I learn the name of a new student, a fact about Nilo-Saharan flies out of my head.” Although his mind was as sharp as ever, age did slow Joe down. He no longer scampered down the stairs from his office. He shuffled ever more slowly from home to Green Library and back. He even stopped working in the library on Saturdays in the last decade of his life, going in “only” five days a week, and stopped working at home at night (!). In his seventies he was unhappy that he would read a gram- mar of a language and not remember everything in it. He complained that he shouldn’t have waited until the age of 65 to start learning Japanese, but at 85 admitted he could read an Ainu-Japanese dictionary without that much difficulty. When he reviewed his African notebooks at the end of life, over four decades after he wrote them, he was disappointed that he couldn’t remember the specific word forms.

OCR for page 152
176 BIOGRAPHICAL MEMOIRS “I learned more from languages than from linguists,” Joe used to say. He was first and foremost an empirical sci- entist of language. Both his controversial work on language universals and his even more controversial work on genetic classification were based on the same method: a nearly exhaustive examination of all the linguistic data he could get his hands on. His language universals and genetic clas- sification—dramatic and far beyond what anyone else had done as they are—were always presented as provisional and subject to revision. Joe was also a very kind-hearted and generous soul. He always lent me his notebooks, even the notebook on which his famous word order paper was based. He lent his Indo- Pacific notebooks to a student who wanted to reanalyze his classification; fortunately, they were returned. Joe was also remarkably cheerful, although he was very hurt by the numer- ous ad hominem attacks on his Amerind classification by the various political machinations in the Stanford linguistics and anthropology departments and by the premature death of his last student, Keith Denning, in 1998. After Joe was diagnosed with cancer he told me he was depressed and added that it was the first time in his life that he had felt depressed. He was devoted to his wife, Selma, to whom he was married for over 60 years, and who was his greatest support throughout his extraordinary career. Selma outlived Joe by over five years; she died in Palo Alto, California on January 28, 2007.

OCR for page 152
177 JOSEPH HAROLD GREENBERG I would like to thank Paul Newman, Merritt Ruhlen, Michael Silver- stein, and Selma Greenberg for their help in preparing this mem- oir. I would also like to thank John Rawlings of Stanford University Library for making available to me a transcript of two interviews he conducted with Greenberg in March 2001. And of course my great- est thanks are to Joe Greenberg himself for sharing the unpublished reminiscences and thoughts reported here, and above all for my education in linguistics. REFERENCES Boas, F., ed. 1911. Handbook of American Indian Languages, Part 1. Smithsonian Institution Bureau of American Ethnology Bulletin 40-1. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office. Boas, F., ed. 1922. Handbook of American Indian Languages, Part 2. Smithsonian Institution Bureau of American Ethnology Bulletin 40-2. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office. Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., A. Piazza, P. Menozzi, and J. Mountain. 1988. Reconstruction of human evolution: Bringing together genetic, archeological and linguistic data. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 85:6002-6006. Croft, W. 2005a. Editor’s introduction. Genetic Linguistics: Theory and Method, Joseph H. Greenberg, pp. x-xxxv. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Croft, W. 2005b. Bibliography of works related to Joseph H. Green- berg’s theory and methods for genetic linguistics. Genetic Linguistics: Theory and Method, Joseph H. Greenberg, pp. 389-410. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Greenberg, J. H. 1986. On being a linguistic anthropologist. Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 15:1-24. Greenberg, J. H. 1994. The influence of Word and the Linguistic Circle of New york on my intellectual development. Word 45:19-25. Greenberg, J. H. 1996a. The genesis of multilateral comparison. Mother Tongue 2:145-148. Greenberg, J. H. 1996b. Genes, languages and other things: Review of History and Geography of Human Genes by L. L. Cavalli-Sforza, P. Menozzi, and A. Piazza. Rev. Archaeol. 16(2):24-28. Lass, R. 1990. How to do things with junk: Exaptation in language change. J. Linguist. 26:79-102.

OCR for page 152
178 BIOGRAPHICAL MEMOIRS McCawley, J. D. 1970. English as a VSO language. Language 46:286- 299. Newman, P. 1995. On being right: Greenberg’s African linguistic classification and the methodological principles which underlie it. Bloomington: Institute for the Study of Nigerian Languages and Cultures, African Studies Program, Indiana University.

OCR for page 152
179 JOSEPH HAROLD GREENBERG SELECTED BIBLIOGRAPHy 1950 The patterning of root morphemes in Semitic. Word 6:162-181. 1953 Historical linguistics and unwritten languages. In Anthropology Today, ed. A. L. Kroeber, pp. 265-286. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. 1954 [1] A quantitative approach to the morphological typology of lan- guage. In Method and Perspective in Anthropology, ed. R. F. Spencer, pp. 192-220. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. [2] Concerning inferences from linguistic to nonlinguistic data. In Language in Culture, ed. H. Hoijer, pp. 3-18. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. 1955 Studies in African Linguistic Classification. New Haven, Conn.: Com- pass Press. 1957 Essays in Linguistics. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. 1963 [1] The Languages of Africa. Bloomington: Indiana University Press. [2] Some universals of grammar with particular reference to the order of meaningful elements. In Universals of Grammar, ed. J. H. Greenberg, pp. 73-113. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. 1966 Language Universals, With Special Reference to Feature Hierarchies. Janua Linguarum, Series Minor 59. The Hague: Mouton.

OCR for page 152
180 BIOGRAPHICAL MEMOIRS 1969 Some methods of dynamic comparison in linguistics. In Substance and Structure of Language, ed. J. Puhvel, pp. 147-203. Berkeley: University of California Press. 1970 Some generalizations concerning glottalic consonants, especially implosives. Int. J. Am. Linguist. 36:123-145. 1971 [1] The Indo-Pacific hypothesis. Curr. Trends Linguist. 8:808-871. [2] Language, Culture and Communication, Essays by Joseph Greenberg, selected and introduced by A. S. Dil. Stanford: Stanford Univer- sity Press. 1972 Numeral classifiers and substantival number: Problems in the gen- esis of a linguistic type. WPLU 9:1-39. Reprinted in Linguistics at the Crossroads ed. Adam Makkai. Padua: Livinia Editrice, 1977 pp. 276-300. 1974 Language Typology: A Historical and Analytic Overview . Janua Linguarum, Series Minor 184. The Hague: Mouton. 1978 With C. A. Ferguson and E. A. Moravcsik, eds. Universals of Human Language. 4 vols. Stanford: Stanford University Press. 1979 Rethinking linguistics diachronically. Language 55:275-290. 1980 Circumfixes and typological change. In Papers from the 4th International Conference on Historical Linguistics, ed. E. C. Traugott et al., pp. 233-241. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

OCR for page 152
181 JOSEPH HAROLD GREENBERG 1986 With C. G. Turner II and S. Zegura. The settlement of the Americas: A comparison of the linguistic, dental and genetic evidence. Curr. Anthropol. 25:477-497. 1987 Language in the Americas. Stanford, Calif.: Stanford University Press. 1990 On Language: Selected Writings of Joseph H. Greenberg, eds. K. Denning and S. Kemmer. Stanford: Stanford University Press. 1991 The last stages of grammatical elements: Contractive and expansive desemanticization. In Approaches to Grammaticalization, eds. E. Trau- gott and B. Heine, pp. 301-314. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 2000 [1] Indo-European and Its Closest Relatives: The Eurasiatic Language Fam- ily, vol. 1, Grammar. Stanford, Calif.: Stanford University Press. [2] From first to second person: The history of Amerind *k(i). In Functional Approaches to Language, Culture and Cognition, eds. D. G. Lockwood, P. H. Fries, and J. E. Copeland, pp. 413-425. Amster- dam: John Benjamins. 2002 Indo-European and Its Closest Relatives: The Eurasiatic Language Family, vol. 2, Lexicon. Stanford: Stanford University Press. 2005 Genetic Linguistics: Essays on Theory and Method, ed. W. Croft. Oxford: Oxford University Press.