National Academies Press: OpenBook
« Previous: 6 Assembling the Pieces and Integrating Elements
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

A.1

NEUROCOGNITIVE MECHANISMS IMPLICATED IN INCREASING THE RISK FOR VIOLENCE

R. James R. Blair1
1 National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, MD

Correspondence to:
R. James R. Blair, PhD
9000 Rockville Pike
Bldg. 15k, Room 205, MSC 2670
Bethesda, MD 20892
jamesblair@mail.nih.gov

This work was supported by the Intramural Research Program of the National Institute of Mental Health, National Institutes of Health, under grant number 1-ZIA-MH002860 to James Blair.

The authors report no conflicts of interest.

Introduction:
Distinguishing Forms of Violence

The goal of this brief review is to consider neurocognitive mechanisms that, when dysfunctional, have been suggested to increase the risk for violence. However, before this can be considered it is worth noting that, from a neuroscience perspective, there appears to be more than one form of violence (Blair, 2001). Specifically, a distinction should be drawn between reactive (affective/defensive/impulsive) and instrumental (proactive/planned) aggression (Crick and Dodge, 1996).

Reactive aggression is unplanned and can be characterized as impulsive. It is explosive, involves the active confrontation of the victim and is typically accompanied by negative affect (anger, sadness, frustration, and irritation). One notable feature distinguishing reactive aggression in humans from that studied in animals is that in humans, it is often associated with frustration (Berkowitz, 1993). Frustration occurs when an individual continues to do an action in the expectation of a reward but does not actually receive that reward (Berkowitz, 1993).

Instrumental aggression in contrast is planned. It involves the selection of a behavior (a covert or overt aggressive response) in anticipation of a positive outcome (e.g., acquisition of territory or goods, improvement of

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

social status, or gratification of a perceived need). Typically, there is an absence of accompanying intense emotion.

Different psychiatric conditions are associated with risks for different forms of aggression. Thus, patients with mood and anxiety conditions (e.g., posttraumatic stress disorder [PTSD]), as well as patients with intermittent explosive disorder (IED) and borderline personality disorder (BPD), are at increased risk for reactive aggression. In contrast, individuals with the personality disorder psychopathy, who show reduced guilt and empathy, show an increased risk for instrumental aggression coupled with an increased risk for reactive aggression (Frick et al., 2005). Importantly, a common pathophysiology likely underpins the increased risk for reactive aggression in PTSD, IED, and BPD (even if there are other aspects of pathophysiology that are idiosyncratic to the individual disorders). In contrast, a rather different pathophysiology likely underpins the increased risk for instrumental aggression in individuals with psychopathic traits.

A System Mediating Reactive Aggression:
Acute Threat Response

The acute threat response involves freezing to a distal threat, fleeing if the threat approaches, and fighting if the threat is very proximal (Blanchard et al., 1977). As such, reactive aggression can be considered the ultimate response to extreme threat. Considerable work with animals has determined that the acute threat response is mediated by a neural circuit that runs from the medial amygdala downward, largely via the stria terminalis to the medial hypothalamus, and then the dorsal half of the periaqueductal gray (Panksepp, 1998; Gregg and Siegel, 2001). This circuitry is assumed to mediate reactive aggression in humans as well (Blair, 2001). In a healthy individual, a very high level of threat might initiate reactive aggression. However, it is suggested that certain clinical conditions lead to lower levels of threat having the same consequence. This is because prior priming of the circuitry, as a consequence of the clinical condition, means that a less intense threat is necessary to initiate reactive aggression (Blair, 2001).

Several psychiatric conditions show a significantly increased risk for reactive aggression. These include PTSD, IED, and BPD (Coccaro et al., 2007; New et al., 2009). In line with the suggestions above of a lowered threshold, patients with these clinical conditions all show heightened responsiveness of regions implicated in reactive aggression, particularly the amygdala, to emotional provocation (Coccaro et al., 2007; Lee et al., 2008; New et al., 2009). In addition, patients with BPD have also been found to show an increased amygdala response to interpersonal provocation (New et al., 2009).

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

Several psychosocial stressors, such as exposure to trauma and neglect, are known to selectively increase the risk for reactive aggression in humans (Crick and Dodge, 1996). Notably, these stressors have also been shown to increase the amygdala response to threat (McCrory et al., 2011; Tottenham et al., 2011). In short, it can be argued that these stressors increase the risk for reactive aggression because they increase the responsiveness of systems mediating the acute threat response. Because of this, the individual is more likely to display reactive aggression in response to future provocation.

Neurocognitive Mechanism That, When Dysfunctional, Increases the Risk for Instrumental Aggression:
Empathic Responsiveness

An association between reduced empathic responsiveness to the distress of others and an increased risk for aggression has long been made (Miller and Eisenberg, 1988). Work has shown that empathic responses to the distress of others diminish aggressive responding (Perry and Perry, 1974). Moreover, empathic responding is critical for socialization. Caregivers most typically respond to transgressions that harm others by focusing on the distress of the victim (Nucci and Nucci, 1982). When this socialization is successful, the individual is less likely to choose actions that will harm others because of the aversive feelings he or she experiences in relation to the anticipated victim’s distress.

Different definitions of empathy have been provided. But the functional processes, with respect to modulating the risk for aggression, concern the impact of distress cues (fearful, sad, and pained facial and vocal expressions) on (1) current behavior by interrupting it; and (2) future behavior by guiding, through social learning, the individual away from behavioral choices associated with harm to others. The amygdala is considered to be critical for both these processes. Considerable data show that the amygdala is responsive to fearful expressions (Adolphs, 2010) and, to a lesser extent, sad and pained expressions. Activation of the amygdala initiates freezing—interrupting current behavior. In addition, the amygdala is important for social learning. The amygdala enables the association of stimuli with the aversive reinforcement engendered by the fear of others (Jeon and Shin, 2011). This social learning means that the healthy individual comes to value actions that harm other individuals as aversive (Blair, 2013).

Deficient empathy is associated with conduct disorder (CD), particularly that associated with psychopathic traits, including reduced guilt (Blair, 2013). Such patients show reduced physiological responsiveness to expressions of distress in peers (Blair, 1999; de Wied et al., 2012). In addition, they show reduced amygdala responses to fearful and sad expressions, as well as the pain of others (Marsh et al., 2008; Viding et al., 2012; White

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

et al., 2012). The presence of psychopathic traits, particularly reduced guilt and empathy, are associated with an increased risk for instrumental aggression (Cornell et al., 1996). The suggestion is that the individual with psychopathic traits is less likely to stop aggressing in response to the victim’s distress because there is less amygdala activity and consequent freezing. In addition, such an individual is less guided away from actions that harm others because he or she has less learned the aversive value of actions that harm others. In line with this, individuals with psychopathic traits have been found to show greater indifference to transgressions that harm others (Aharoni, Antonenko, and Kiehl, 2011; Blair, 1995) and reduced amygdala responses to such transgressions (Glenn, Raine, and Schug, 2008). Such indifference means that the individual is more likely to commit an action that will harm others to achieve his or her goals.

Neurocognitive Mechanism That, When Dysfunctional, Increases the Risk for Reactive and Instrumental Aggression:
Reward–Punishment-Based Decision Making

Problems in reward–punishment-based decision making are likely to increase the risk for reactive and instrumental aggression in several ways. First, individuals who fail to learn how to obtain rewards and avoid punishments through their decisions face high risk for impulsivity and frustration. Both impulsivity and frustration, in turn, predict risk for reactive aggression (Berkowitz, 1974). Second, it is suggested that systems involved in decision making are also involved in the regulation of the acute threat response. Specifically, ventromedial frontal cortex is involved in the selection of appropriate action as a function of the value outcomes associated with different behavioral choices (a critical component of reward–punishment-based decision making). An angry reactive response to provocation may not be selected if the negative value of the detrimental consequences of this action is appropriately represented (e.g., costs of imprisonment). Third, and relatedly, instrumental aggressive responses will be less likely to be selected if the negative values of their detrimental consequences are represented (e.g., victim’s distress, and costs of imprisonment).

Systems neuroscience research links specific forms of decision making, involving reinforcement-based learning, to a circuit that connects the striatum, amygdala, and ventromedial frontal cortex as well as other structures (O’Doherty, 2012). Patients with conduct disorder exhibit a form of impaired decision making associated with dysfunction in this neural circuit. For example, they are more likely to respond to stimuli that engender punishment or continue to respond to stimuli that, while once rewarding, now engender punishment (Crowley et al., 2010; Finger et al., 2008; White et al.,

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

2013). Notably, such forms of impaired decision making occur in conduct disorder either with or without high levels of callous and unemotional traits (White et al., 2013), as well as in patients with ADHD without conduct disorder (Plichta et al., 2009; Scheres, Milham, Knutson, and Castellanos, 2007; Strohle et al., 2008). Moreover, they may occur in unaffected but at-risk children, born to parents with their own histories of conduct problems or drug addiction (Yau et al., 2012). Thus, impaired decision making arising from fronto-striatal dysfunction may represent a shared substrate for frequently comorbid disorders, such as conduct disorder, ADHD, and substance use disorders.

Conclusions

In conclusion, this review outlines three neurocognitive systems that, when dysfunctional, increase the risk for aggression. These are (1) the acute threat response implicating the amygdala, hypothalamus, and periaqueductal gray; (2) empathic responding (instrumental) implicating the amygdala and, in the context of decision making influenced by empathy for potential victims, ventromedial frontal cortex; and (3) reward–punishment-based decision making implicating striatum and ventromedial frontal cortex. Importantly, identifying these systems provides treatment targets. Interventions can be designed to address the functioning of these systems and their efficacy, indexed by their impact on the systems themselves, as well as downstream consequences of reduced aggression.

References

Adolphs, R. (2010). What does the amygdala contribute to social cognition? Ann N Y Acad Sci, 1191, 42-61. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05445.x.

Aharoni, E., Antonenko, O., and Kiehl, K. A. (2011). Disparities in the moral intuitions of criminal offenders: The role of psychopathy. J Res Pers, 45(3), 322-327. doi: 10.1016/j. jrp.2011.02.005.

Berkowitz, L. (1993). Aggression: Its causes, consequences, and control. Philadelphia, PA: Temple University Press.

Berkowitz, L. (1974). Some determinants of impulsive aggression: Role of mediated associations with reinforcements for aggression. Psychological Review, 81, 165-176.

Blair, R. J. R. (1995). A cognitive developmental approach to morality: Investigating the psychopath. Cognition, 57, 1-29.

Blair, R. J. R. (2001). Neurocognitive models of aggression, the antisocial personality disorders, and psychopathy. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 71(6), 727-731.

Blair, R. J. (2013). The neurobiology of psychopathic traits in youths. Nat Rev Neurosci, 14(11), 786-799. doi: 10.1038/nrn3577.

Blair, R. J. R. (1999). Responsiveness to distress cues in the child with psychopathic tendencies. Personality and Individual Differences, 27, 135-145.

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

Blanchard, R. J., Blanchard, D. C., and Takahashi, L. K. (1977). Attack and defensive behaviour in the albino rat. Animal Behavior, 25, 197-224.

Blanchard, R. J., Takahashi, L. K., and Blanchard, D. C. (1977). The development of intruder attack in colonies of laboratory rats. Animal Learning and Behavior, 5(4), 365-369.

Coccaro, E. F., McCloskey, M. S., Fitzgerald, D. A., and Phan, K. L. (2007). Amygdala and orbitofrontal reactivity to social threat in individuals with impulsive aggression. Biological Psychiatry, 62(2), 168-178.

Cornell, D. G., Warren, J., Hawk, G., Stafford, E., Oram, G., and Pine, D. (1996). Psychopathy in instrumental and reactive violent offenders. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 783-790.

Crick, N. R., and Dodge, K. A. (1996). Social information-processing mechanisms in reactive and proactive aggression. Child Development, 67(3), 993-1002.

Crowley, T. J., Dalwani, M. S., Mikulich-Gilbertson, S. K., Du, Y. P., Lejuez, C. W., Raymond, K. M., and Banich, M. T. (2010). Risky decisions and their consequences: Neural processing by boys with Antisocial Substance Disorder. PLoS ONE, 5(9), e12835. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0012835.

de Wied, M., van Boxtel, A., Matthys, W., and Meeus, W. (2012). Verbal, facial and autonomic responses to empathy-eliciting film clips by disruptive male adolescents with high versus low callous-unemotional traits. J Abnorm Child Psychol, 40(2), 211-223. doi: 10.1007/s10802-011-9557-8.

Finger, E. C., Marsh, A. A., Mitchell, D. G. V., Reid, M. E., Sims, C., Budhani, S., . . . Blair, R. J. R. (2008). Abnormal ventromedial prefrontal cortex function in children with psychopathic traits during reversal learning. Archives of General Psychiatry, 65(5), 586-594.

Frick, P. J., Stickle, T. R., Dandreaux, D. M., Farrell, J. M., and Kimonis, E. R. (2005). Callous-unemotional traits in predicting the severity and stability of conduct problems and delinquency. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 33(4), 471-487.

Glenn, A. L., Raine, A., and Schug, R. A. (2008). The neural correlates of moral decision-making in psychopathy. Molecular Psychiatry, 14, 5-6.

Gregg, T. R., and Siegel, A. (2001). Brain structures and neurotransmitters regulating aggression in cats: Implications for human aggression. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biological Psychiatry, 25(1), 91-140.

Jeon, D., and Shin, H. S. (2011). A mouse model for observational fear learning and the empathetic response. Curr Protoc Neurosci, Chapter 8, Unit 8 27. doi: 10.1002/0471142301. ns0827s57.

Lee, T. M. C., Chan, S. C., and Raine, A. (2008). Strong limbic and weak frontal activation to aggressive stimuli in spouse abusers. Molecular Psychiatry, 13(7), 655-656.

Marsh, A. A., Finger, E. C., Mitchell, D. G. V., Reid, M. E., Sims, C., Kosson, D. S., . . . Blair, R. J. R. (2008). Reduced amygdala response to fearful expressions in children and adolescents with callous-unemotional traits and disruptive behavior disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165(6), 712-720.

McCrory, E. J., De Brito, S. A., Sebastian, C. L., Mechelli, A., Bird, G., Kelly, P. A., and Viding, E. (2011). Heightened neural reactivity to threat in child victims of family violence. Curr Biol, 21(23), R947-948. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.10.015.

Miller, P. A., and Eisenberg, N. (1988). The relation of empathy to aggressive and externalizing/antisocial behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 324-344.

New, A. S., Hazlett, E. A., Newmark, R. E., Zhang, J., Triebwasser, J., Meyerson, D., Lazarus, S., Trisdorfer, R., Goldstein, K. E., Goodman, M., Koenigsberg, H. W., Flory, J. D., Siever, L. J., and Buchsbaum, M. S. (2009). Laboratory induced aggression: A positron emission tomography study of aggressive individuals with borderline personality disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 66(12), 1107-1114.

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

Nucci, L. P., and Nucci, M. (1982). Children’s social interactions in the context of moral and conventional transgressions. Child Development, 53, 403-412.

O’Doherty, J. P. (2012). Beyond simple reinforcement learning: The computational neurobiology of reward-learning and valuation. Eur J Neurosci, 35(7), 987-990. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.08074.x.

Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. New York: Oxford University Press.

Perry, D. G., and Perry, L. C. (1974). Denial of suffering in the victim as a stimulus to violence in aggressive boys. Child Development, 45, 55-62.

Plichta, M. M., Vasic, N., Wolf, R. C., Lesch, K. P., Brummer, D., Jacob, C., . . . Gron, G. (2009). Neural hyporesponsiveness and hyperresponsiveness during immediate and delayed reward processing in adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry, 65(1), 7-14. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.07.008.

Scheres, A., Milham, M. P., Knutson, B., and Castellanos, F. X. (2007). Ventral striatal hyporesponsiveness during reward anticipation in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry, 61(5), 720-724. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.04.042.

Strohle, A., Stoy, M., Wrase, J., Schwarzer, S., Schlagenhauf, F., Huss, M., . . . Heinz, A. (2008). Reward anticipation and outcomes in adult males with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuroimage, 39(3), 966-972. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.09.044.

Tottenham, N., Hare, T. A., Millner, A., Gilhooly, T., Zevin, J. D., and Casey, B. J. (2011). Elevated amygdala response to faces following early deprivation. Dev Sci, 14(2), 190-204. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00971.x.

Viding, E., Sebastian, C. L., Dadds, M. R., Lockwood, P. L., Cecil, C. A., De Brito, S. A., and McCrory, E. J. (2012). Amygdala response to preattentive masked fear in children with conduct problems: The role of callous-unemotional traits. Am J Psychiatry, 169(10), 1109-1116. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12020191.

White, S. F., Marsh, A. A., Fowler, K. A., Schechter, J. C., Adalio, C., Pope, K., . . . Blair, R. J. R. (2012). Reduced amygdala responding in youth with disruptive behavior disorder and psychopathic traits reflects a reduced emotional response not increased top-down attention to non-emotional features. American Journal of Psychiatry, 169(7), 750-758.

White, S. F., Pope, K., Sinclair, S., Fowler, K. A., Brislin, S. J., Williams, W. C., . . . Blair, R. J. R. (2013). Disrupted expected value and prediction error signaling in youth with disruptive behavior disorders during a passive avoidance task. American Journal of Psychiatry.

Yau, W. Y., Zubieta, J. K., Weiland, B. J., Samudra, P. G., Zucker, R. A., and Heitzeg, M. M. (2012). Nucleus accumbens response to incentive stimuli anticipation in children of alcoholics: Relationships with precursive behavioral risk and lifetime alcohol use. J Neurosci, 32(7), 2544-2551. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.1390-11.2012.

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

A.2

VIOLENCE AND MENTAL HEALTH:
OPPORTUNITIES FOR PREVENTION AND EARLY INTERVENTION, A WORKSHOP OF THE NATIONAL ACADEMIES OF SCIENCES, ENGINEERING, AND MEDICINE’S FORUM ON GLOBAL VIOLENCE PREVENTION, FEBRUARY 26, 2014

Daniel Fisher

Presentation by Daniel Fisher, M.D.,
Executive Director, National Empowerment Center,
25 Bigelow St., Cambridge, MA 02139

The very title of this conference saddens me, and makes me angry. Clearly the gun lobby has been effective in changing the narrative from controlling guns to controlling those of us who have been labeled mentally ill. This narrative is based on false information equating persons with mental health disorders with increased violence.

Introduction

In my 20s I was diagnosed with schizophrenia and was involuntarily hospitalized on three occasions. Ironically, I was studying the possible biochemical bases of mental illnesses at the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) at the time. (That was in the late ’60s. In May of last year, 45 years later, Dr. Thomas Insel of NIMH stated that NIMH will not use the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition [DSM-5] because its diagnostic categories are not based on biological markers.) I was researching the factors controlling the biosynthesis of dopamine and serotonin. I so reduced human experiences to chemistry that I became convinced that we all were merely chemical machines, and that we lacked meaning or agency. I lost the meaning of human relationships and emotional expression. Being unable to understand communication left me out of touch. I believe this empty view of life and the loneliness it produced left me in despair and caused me to depart from everyday reality. I indeed did fit the DSM definition of schizophrenia. I recovered from schizophrenia by finding meaning through emotionally connecting with others and myself. I concluded that we have a self that supersedes all we can write in a formula. This understanding brought me back to everyday life with people. I decided I could learn more about this human dimension by becoming a psychiatrist,

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

and have practiced in clinical settings for the last 35 years. I also founded and run the National Empowerment Center, a Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (SAMHSA)-funded, technical assistance center dedicated to bringing hope and recovery to the mental health system and society.

Recovery depends on people with mental illness finding meaningful relationships and working in the community. The misinformation perpetuated by our media is interfering with recovery. This incorrect coupling of mental health issues with violence increases prejudice and discrimination among all members of society. (Advocates are rejecting the use of the term “stigma” because the term itself has produced increased prejudice.)

I will summarize the evidence that people diagnosed with mental health conditions are no more violent than matched community members without such a label. Although there is a lack of association of violence and mental health conditions, the increased attention on mental health can be an opportunity to improve that system. Therefore, I will point out problems in our mental health system and society that are barriers to recovery. Finally, I will recommend ways that our mental health system needs to continue its transformation from a maintenance-based to a recovery-based system.

The Evidence Does Not Link Persons with Mental Health Disorders to Violence

A recently published report by the Consortium for Risk-Based Firearm Policy (2013) concluded: “Research evidence shows that the large majority of people with mental illness do not engage in violence against others and most violence is caused by factors other than mental illness.” The report also found that “research evidence suggests that . . . mental illness alone rarely causes violence.” These conclusions were based on three studies (Elbogen and Johnson, 2009; Swanson et al., 2013; Van Dorn et al., 2012).

This evidence fits closely with the findings of the MacArthur Violence Risk Assessment Study, considered one of the most definitive published studies of mental health issues and violence. Dr. Heather Stuart (summarized the MacArthur Study as follows: The MacArthur Violence Risk Assessment Study (Applebaum et al., 2000; Monahan et al., 2001; Steadman et al., 1998, 2000) “has made a concerted effort to address . . . [methodological] problems, so it stands out as the most sophisticated attempt to date to disentangle these complex interrelationships” (Stuart, 2003).

Because they collected extensive follow-up data on a large cohort of subject (N = 1,136), the temporal sequencing of important events is clear. Because they used multiple measures of violence, including patient self-report, they have minimized the information bias characterizing past work. The innovative use of same-neighbor comparison subjects eliminates confound-

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

ing from broad environmental influences such as socio-demographic or economic factors that may have exaggerated differences in past research.

In this study, the prevalence of violence among those with a major mental disorder who did not abuse substances was indistinguishable from their non-substance abusing neighborhood controls. Delusions were not associated with violence, even ‘threat control override’ delusions that cause an individual to think that someone is out to harm them or that someone can control their thoughts. (Stuart, 2003)

Fazel and colleagues (Fazel et al., 2009a), carried out a meta-analysis of 20 studies that examined a possible relationship of violence to mental health conditions. They concluded that “psychosis comorbid with substance abuse confers no additional risk over and above the risk associated with substance abuse.” This finding was consistent with their own finding that schizophrenia, in the absence of substance abuse, did not increase the risk of violence when compared to the general population (Fazel et al., 2009b).

Therefore, every significant research study carried out starting with the MacArthur Study in the late 1990s has concluded that:

  1. Persons with mental illness are no more likely than the matched controls in the community [to perform violent acts].
  2. Persons with a substance abuse disorder carry a substantial risk of increased violence.

Therefore, “Strategies that aim to reduce gun violence by focusing . . . on restricting access to guns by those diagnosed with a mental illness are unlikely to reduce the overall rate of gun violence in the United States” (Consortium for Risk-Based Firearm Policy, 2013).

The National Instant Background Check System, NICS, should be focused on dangerousness and a history of violence rather than a mental health diagnosis per se (Consortium for Risk-Based Firearm Policy, 2013). After all, those of us diagnosed with a mental health disorder account for only 4 percent of the gun-related homicides (Swanson et al., 2013).

Our Mental Health System Is Not a Welcoming Place

It is dehumanizing, and hope-robbing approaches are major reasons why it is broken. It could practice hospitality, like hotels, through the practice of dialogue. Instead it practices varying degrees of coercion, persuasion, and suggestion. These all are forms of trauma. To be trauma-informed, we need to adhere to principles of dialogue emphasizing connecting, especially at the emotional level, mutual respect for the full humanity of the person

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

in distress and listening to his or her voice, believing in them, and giving them hope and humility. Peers can greatly enhance these qualities when their roles as recovery coaches are valued and understood.

Our Deeper Malaise

Rather than focus on our mental health system alone, I want to point out that dehumanizing labels and coercion are a reflection of a deeper woe, that is, the breakdown of cohesion in our communities. The fear in our society of people who are different or odd is the basis for such reactions. We desperately need to rebuild positive connections to each other in our communities. Cross-cultural studies have shown that contact with people of differing race and culture, as well as mental health issues, is a critical factor in becoming more comfortable with them. Contact also decreases prejudice, stigma, and discrimination.

Recommendations

  1. Broaden the community dialogue on mental health, and ensure that persons with lived experience of mental health conditions are included in the planning and participation of these dialogues.
  2. NIMH and SAMHSA should promote the training and evaluation of Open Dialogue (Seikkula, 2006) in the United States, to reach people where they live, and while they are still connected to their natural supports. This approach, developed in Finland, is the most successful approach in the world for helping young people who have experienced their first psychotic experience to recover a full life in the community.
  3. Hiring peers in valued roles as crisis workers and in peer-run respites; peers are capable of reaching persons whom non-peers cannot reach. This is true because when you have experienced delusions and voices, you know how to reach and connect with other persons going through a similar experience. Peer-run respites are a good example of the application of this capacity of peers in diverting persons from hospitalization (see the section on crisis respites at www.power2u.org).
  4. Training first responders, peers, and families in Emotional CPR. This is a preventative public health program, enabling anyone to help another person through an emotional crisis. eCPR, therefore, represents the type of primary prevention that would reach a much greater proportion of persons than present programs that focus on persons labeled with mental health disorders (see www.emotionalcpr.org).
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
  1. Speakers sharing recovery stories in person with media officials, police, parents, and other important community groups. Research has shown that the most effective way to reduce prejudice and discrimination is through people sharing their stories (Corrigan, 2005).
  2. Educating the media to not continue the misinformation of links between mental health issues and violence (the “See Me” campaign in Scotland is a good example of this media education).
  3. Continue the transformation from a medical narrative to a recovery narrative that was started in the Surgeon General’s Mental Health Report (1999) and the New Freedom Commission (Fisher, 2008; New Freedom Commission on Mental Health, 2003).

Comments on Other Talks Given at the National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine’s Workshop on Violence and Mental Health

Dr. Paul Appelbaum pointed out that Wayne LaPierre, vice president of the National Rifle Association (NRA), quickly shifted the blame for the Sandy Hook shootings in December 2012 from guns to persons labeled with mental illness. He said that the problem of gun violence is not guns but people with mental illness, whom he described as “deranged mongrels.” The NRA called for ensuring that persons labeled with mental illness be placed on the National Instant Criminal Background Check System (NICS). This represented a change in policy of the NRA, which as recently as 2007, had loosened the NICS: “The price for bringing the NRA on board [for the NICS bill of 2007] was to take the ‘mentally ill’ tag away from anyone ‘rehabilitated through any procedure available under law’ and to enact a ‘Relief from Disabilities’ reform. The latter reform allowed people classified as mentally ill, and unable to buy guns, to get their rights back with more ease” (Weigel, 2013).

References

Appelbaum, P.S., Robbins, P.C., Monahan, J. 2000. Violence and delusions: Data from the MacArthur Violence Risk Assessment Study. Am. J. Psychiatry 157:566-572.

Consortium for Risk-Based Firearm Policy. 2013. Guns, public health and mental illness: An evidence-based approach for state policy. John Hopkins University: Baltimore, MD.

Corrigan, P. 2005. On the stigma of mental illness: Implications for research and social change, fifth edition. American Psychological Association: Washington, DC.

Elbogen, E., and Johnson, S. 2009. Intricate link between violence and mental disorder results from national epidemiological survey on alcohol and related conditions. Archives General Psychiatry 66:152-161.

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

Fazel, S., Gulati, G., Linsell, L., Geddes, J.R., and Grann, M. 2009. Schizophrenia and violence: Systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS Med 6(8): e1000120. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.1000120.

Fazel, S., Låndström, N., Hjern, A., Grann, M., and Lichtenstein, P. 2009b. Schizophrenia, substance abuse, and violent crime. JAMA 301(19):2016-2023.

Fisher, D.B. 2008. Promoting recovery. Learning about mental health practice. Eds. T. Stickley and T. Basset. John Wiley and Sons: Chichester, U.K. pp. 119-139.

McGinty, E., et al. 2013. Gun policy and serious mental illness: Priorities for future research and policy. Psychiatric Services, epub ahead of print, doi:10.1176/appi. Ps.201300141.

Monahan, J., Steadman, H.J., Silver, E., et al. 2001. Risk assessment: the MacArthur Study of Mental Disorder and Violence. Oxford University Press: Oxford, U.K.

New Freedom Commission on Mental Health. 2003. Achieving the promise: Transforming mental health care in America final report. DHHS Pub. No. SMA-03-3832. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services: Rockville, MD.

Seikkula, J., and Trimble, D. 2005. Healing elements of therapeutic conversation: Dialogue as an embodiment of love. Family Process 44(4):461-475.

Seikkula, J., Aaltonen, K., Alakare, B., Haarakanga, K., Keranen, J., and Lehtinen, K. 2006. Five-year experience of first-episode nonaffective psychosis in open-dialogue approach: Treatment principles, follow-up outcomes, and two case studies. Psychotherapy Research 16(2):214-228.

Steadman, H.J., Silver, E., Monahan, J., Appelbaum, P.S., et al. 2000. A classification tree approach to the development of actuarial violence risk-assessment tools. Law Humanity Behavior 24:83-100.

Steadman, H.J., Mulvy, E.P., Monahan, J., et al. 1998. Violence by people discharged from acute psychiatric inpatient facilities and by others in the same neighborhoods. Arch Gen Psychiatry 55:393-404.

Stuart, H. 2003. Violence and mental illness: An overview. World Psychiatric Assoc 2:121-124.

Swanson, J. et al. 2013. “Preventing gun violence involving people with serious mental illness.” In Reducing gun violence in America: Informing policy with evidence and analysis. Webster, D. and Vernick, J. eds. Johns Hopkins University Press: Baltimore, MD. pp. 33-51.

U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. 1999. Mental health: A report of the Surgeon General. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, Center for Mental Health Services, National Institutes of Health, National Institute of Mental Health: Rockville, MD.

Van Dorn, R., Volavka, J., and Johnson, N. 2012. Mental disorder and violence: Is there a relationship beyond substance abuse? Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 47:487-503.

Weigel, D. 2013. CPAC Diary: Wayne LaPierre’s “Mental Health” Chutzpah. Slate Magazine, March 15.

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

A.3

INTERFACE WITH THE JUSTICE COMMUNITY: THE POLICE

Sheldon Greenberg, Ph.D.
Johns Hopkins University

Despite the long-term call by criminal justice and mental health professionals, advocates, and political officials to work together more effectively in providing services to people with mental illness, progress in many jurisdictions (cities, counties, towns, states, tribal land) and nations has been slow. This paper presents and expands on a portion of the information presented during the workshop titled Mental Health and Violence: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Intervention. It offers information on the interface between the criminal justice and mental health communities, addresses some of the reasons for measured progress, and suggests ways in which the two professions might advance success. While this paper briefly addresses public safety, including corrections, it focuses primarily on interaction among the police, mental health providers, and people who have mental illness and their families, friends, and other advocates.

For decades, mental health professionals, advocacy organizations, family members, political leaders, and others have sought to have the criminal justice system or criminal justice community (generally perceived as consisting of the police, courts, and corrections) end the unnecessary “criminalization” of mental illness. This continues to be a priority.

Generally, an arrest is defined as the taking of a person into custody by a legal authority, usually in response to a criminal charge. The criminalization hypothesis is based on the assumption that police inappropriately use arrest to resolve encounters with people who have mental illness (Engel and Silver, 2001). Criminalization that can affect the future of a person with mental illness goes beyond traditional arrest and may include criminal citations (usually issued for minor offenses that do not require taking a person into custody) and inclusion of names in criminal incident reports.

Terms such as “criminal justice system” and “criminal justice community” suggest that there is a common foundation of standards, policies, practices, and other linkages across the profession. In fact, criminal justice in the United States and in many other nations is a highly fragmented, parochial, and compartmentalized and, at times, is a competitive collection of local, state, tribal, and federal agencies. There is no central organization, professional association, or other body with the authority to mandate policy, practice, or change. As of 2011, there were approximately 15,000 local police departments in the United States. The majority of these agencies

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

have fewer than 25 personnel. Fewer than 70 agencies serve jurisdictions with large populations (500,000 or more) (Baltic, 2011). Fragmentation exists in other nations as well, although some nations have a single national police force (France, Italy, Japan, New Zealand, Nicaragua) or a small number of agencies (Sweden, United Kingdom), and other nations’ law enforcement services are provided by their defense agency/military (Chilean Carabineros).

For a brief perspective on the larger justice system in the United States, there are 3,365 local jails, not including state, federal, military, or juvenile correctional institutions. Data on the number of courts—federal, state, local, military, and specialized—are weak, although it is estimated that there are more than 3,000 trial court judges serving the nation. The global criminal justice system is diverse, and the concept of justice (fair processes, just outcomes, respect for human rights and dignity, upholding law) is too varied and complex in some nations and ill-defined in others to describe adequately in this paper (Nagel, 2005).

Since the period of deinstitutionalization (intended to close large, often antiquated public mental health hospitals and ensure the transfer of patients to community-based mental health services) that began in the mid-1950s and increased continually through the 1980s, criminal justice agencies have altered policies, procedures, training, and relationships with mental health providers and advocates to overcome difficulties stemming from critical and routine interaction with people who have severe mental illness (Lurigio and Swartz, 2000). When deinstitutionalization began in 1955, there were approximately 560,000 severely mentally ill patients in the nation’s public psychiatric hospitals. By 1994, this number was reduced by more than 485,000 patients to approximately 72,000 (Frontline, 2005). Several failed federal acts and the weak community response to supporting those who returned to the community placed new and immediate burdens on the police, courts, and corrections. Similar experiences occurred in other nations and continents, including Australia, New Zealand, and Western Europe (Fakhoury and Priebe, 2002). Two significant consequences of deinstitutionalization have been the “criminalization of mental illness” (Peternelj-Taylor, 2008) and victimization of people who have mental illness (Teplin et al., 2005).

In considering the interface with the criminal justice system by mental health providers, public health, social services, the faith community, and other organizations, it is essential to focus on the role of the frontline police officer as the “point of entry” and “point of diversion” toward or away from arrest, prosecution, and incarceration. Generally, a police officer’s decision to connect with a mental health service provider or other support service, arrest (criminalize), pursue hospitalization or other placement, or handle the call informally is based primarily on the characteristics and

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

constraints of each situation rather than the symptomatology or awareness of mental illness (Lamb et al., 2002; Teplin, 2000). Action and decisions by prosecutors and the courts will be determined, in great part, by the actions and reports of the responding police officers.

Police agencies have sought to improve response to calls for service and other incidents involving people with mental illness, especially those considered crisis or emergency situations. These new developments (training, advances in policy, scrutiny of arrests, crisis intervention teams), however, have been targeted almost exclusively at improved handling of individual incidents. Less attention has been devoted to developing or implementing comprehensive, multiagency, and preventive approaches (Cordner, 2006).

Generally, police become involved in situations and interact with people who have mental illness when problems surface (Cordner, 2006). One three-city study found that 92 percent of uniformed patrol officers had at least one interaction with a person experiencing a mental health crisis in the previous month. Police officers report that they are involved in an average of six calls per month specifically related to people with mental illness (Borum, 2000; Vermette et al., 2005). An estimated 7 percent of police contacts in jurisdictions with 100,000 or more involve people with mental illness (Deane et al., 1999). The above data are based on reported incidents. A significant percentage of a police officer’s interaction, intervention, and problem-solving activity never makes its way into police reports and is never captured in the data. Such activity is not found in the reported data for several reasons: the situation requiring police intervention may have been resolved quickly or informally; the officer did not want the involved individuals to be named in a report; and/or the information may have been recorded according to the initial call (assault, domestic situation, suspicious activity, injured person), with no identifiers related to mental illness. In some cases, the officer simply may have chosen not to file a formal report.

Research shows consistently that police officers do not want to criminalize events involving people with mental illness unless significant violation of the law has occurred (assault, theft, arson, weapons violation) and cannot be ignored or handled less formally. Officers express frustration in handling calls involving people with mental illness because of the lack of needed information, the lack of immediately available resources, and the lack of coordination in effort between police and mental health professionals (Engel and Silver, 2001; Novak and Engel, 2005; Wells and Schafer, 2006).

There is minimal research on the face-to-face interaction among the first-responding patrol officer, the person who has mental illness, family members, and others. Little is known about what occurs in the first moments of a situation that drives the involved players toward pursuing informal deescalation, or more formal interventions, such as criminalization, a call for response by mental health professionals, commitment/hospitalization, or

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

other interventions or diversions from the criminal justice system (Tucker, Hasselt, and Russell, 2008).

Notable progress was made with the establishment of Crisis Intervention Teams (CITs) in the late 1980s, which brought together well-trained police officers and mental health professionals to respond immediately to calls for service involving people who have mental illness. Although recognized primarily for intervention in serious or critical situations, CITs responded to any call (crisis and non-critical) in which the caller or responding police officer identified the need for support. These teams primarily appeared in urban and metropolitan environments—Baltimore County, Charlotte-Mecklenburg, Los Angeles, Memphis, and Milwaukee—which had the demand and resources to support them. In some of these jurisdictions, police officers received 40 hours of instruction or more on intervention strategies and often participated in joint training with mental health workers.

Officers who received the training and were assigned to CITs reported improved ability to recognize and respond to calls involving people with mental illness, reduced stereotyping, greater empathy, improved ability to resolve the situation, better communication skills, and increased patience. They further reported that they were less likely to arrest and more prone to redirect the individual toward other forms of support (Hanafi et al., 2008). Importantly, research shows that officers trained to participate in CITs or who received comparable training regarding mental health intervention were less likely than their peers, in some situations, to use force or perceive escalating force as effective in dealing with a person who has mental illness (Compton et al., 2009).

Access to CITs in the United States began declining in recent years, along with other support services, due to local budget crises and federal cost containment strategies (Cunningham et al., 2006). Expansion of CITs is questionable for the foreseeable future.

Training for police personnel remains a void in improving criminal justice service to people who have mental illness. Data on the number of police officers trained are unreliable since some agencies train more than those needed to serve on CITs, and others, especially smaller agencies, rely on other organizations to train their employees. Some agencies do not keep comprehensive data on training provided to their employees. Some states and larger agencies have sought to provide mental health intervention training to a minimum of 20 percent of their officers (Compton et al., 2008). To date, there has been minimal research assessing the quality of CIT and other mental health training provided to police officers, its endurance over time, or the impact it has on outcomes for people who have mental illness and their families (Lord et al., 2011).

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

Officers assigned to CITs receive training that surpasses the norm for their peers. A survey of 33 state law enforcement certification agencies (Police Officer Standards and Training Commissions [POSTs]), which exist in almost every state in the United States, showed that the average training for police officers on “mental illness” was 9.1 hours. The majority of training courses ranged from 2 to 4 hours, with the shortest course being 50 minutes. These courses included content on awareness and process (policies and procedures for arrest, safety, and commitment to a hospital, shelter, or other care facility). Some of the courses described by the states incorporated mental illness with training on all “special populations,” with no information on time allotted specifically to mental illness. Two states reported that they have no requirement for training on mental illness. Little research is available on training provided to specialized police agencies, such as school police, campus police and security personnel, transportation police, and tribal police. The inconsistency in and minimal amount of training provided to police on service to people who have mental illness is a global issue (Psarra et al., 2008).

Calls are made to the police by people with mental illness who have been victimized, as well as by spouses, other family members, neighbors, mental health professionals, and people who simply observe behavior or an incident but have no relationship to the person with mental illness. The importance of the initial point of contact—often a police or other government call taker/dispatcher—in obtaining critical information and relaying it to the responding police patrol officer cannot be overstated. Despite improved police call-taking protocols, information provided to the police call taker by the victim or observer (witness) is too often brief, panicked, incomplete, and inaccurate. They may only report the immediate need, threat, or danger and fail to mention that a person who has mental illness is involved. As such, the initial information a police officer receives may make no reference to mental illness or contain any details about risk, existing injury or illness, medication use, illicit substance abuse, presence of weapons, or precise location. Officers need, but often lack, information on the individual’s medical or criminal history, cause of the crisis or hostility, prior suicide or self-injury attempts, and attending physicians, among other information (James, 1990). Information provided to a responding police officer may be described as and limited to the following:

  • Man injured
  • Woman acting out
  • Doctor has trouble with patient
  • Unknown trouble
  • Suspicious circumstance
  • Assault
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
  • Threat of assault
  • Threat of suicide
  • Parent cannot control child
  • Disorderly conduct
  • Assist with a commitment

Officer safety and the safety of others are paramount when police officers receive a call for service or personally observe an unusual behavior. When information about mental illness is conveyed, no matter how detailed, police officers make assumptions about the potential for danger (Watson et al., 2004).

Lack of Information and Need for Research on Initial Points of Contact

Much of what occurs—including police officers’ discretionary decision making to criminalize or divert from criminalization—is based on the observed behavior, early formulation of perceptions, and communication that occurs in the early moments of the situation. Yet, little is known about the nature and quality of the initial face-to-face interaction of police officers called into situations involving people who have mental illness. Conclusions and assumptions about the early one-on-one interaction that occur are drawn from police incident report narratives, interviews, anecdotal information, and, to a lesser degree, statistical reports, policies and procedures, and training curricula.

There is minimal research on the initial face-to-face interaction (point of contact) between police officers and people who have mental illness. There is little research on the initial interaction between responding police officers and spouses, family members, neighbors, and other involved parties. There is little research on how information conveyed early in a situation might influence, positively or negatively, the re-criminalization of a person with mental illness who had prior contact with the criminal justice system. Research is needed to answer key questions that can possibly lead to more positive courses of action for people with mental illness who are victimized, accused, or perceived as being dangerous to themselves or others:

  • What is the level of understanding about the situation by the responding officer before he or she arrives?
  • What facts were conveyed to the police department by the caller, and how was this information relayed to the responding officer?
  • At what point in the situation does awareness of mental illness become known?
  • What co-occurring factors (injury, illicit substance abuse) were apparent at the immediate point of contact?
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
  • What was the real or perceived level of risk of harm (presence of weapons, threats), and how did it evolve?
  • How emotional or tense was the situation upon the officer’s arrival?
  • What words or actions by the dispatcher, family members, witnesses, and people who have mental illness set the course for police action, particularly the criminalizing or decriminalizing of the situation?
  • To what degree did the immediate situation allow for alternatives to criminalization?
  • Was information shared about prior history, including incarceration or hospitalization, and what influence did this have on the initial thinking and action by the responding police officer(s)?
  • Was the availability (readiness at the time of need) of mental health service providers known to the initial responding officer(s)?
  • At what point did police officers connect with a mental health service provider, and what was the nature of their initial information sharing and action planning?

Other Needs to Improve Police Interaction

Other needs and recommendations to improve interaction between the police and people who have mental illness were discussed during a roundtable discussion with public safety practitioners held at The Johns Hopkins University (Division of Public Safety Leadership, 2013). The following reflects some of these needs and recommendations. Due to space limitation, the following recommendations are noted in brief:

  • Improved methods for conveying research to frontline practitioners
  • Research on point-of-contact communication and other characteristics of initial interaction that influence outcomes
  • Orientation/training for mental health professionals on public safety and, particularly, the police (culture, fragmentation, training, policy, safety mandates, discretion, liability)
  • Research on the curricula being used to teach the police, with attention to awareness, interaction, use of community-based resources, and law
  • Model curricula for police, designed to accommodate the various time limitations imposed by police academy schedules
  • Model curricula for police on how to manage concurrent issues (mental illness, mobility or communication disability, criminal behavior, homelessness, substance abuse, injury)
  • Model curricula on interacting with people with mental illness who are victimized
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
  • Research on successes, beyond crisis intervention teams, particularly those related to informal diffusion of situations and those in which criminalization did not occur
  • Model training and awareness programs, supported by marketing strategies, to educate people who have mental illness and their support network (spouse, family, friends) on interacting with the police, particularly during initial contact
  • Advancing call-taker protocols to obtain more and better information from callers to relay to responding police officers and, where joint response occurs, mental health workers or CITs

References

Baltic, S. E. (Ed.). (2011). Crime in the United States 2011. Bernan Press.

Borum, R. (2000). Improving high-risk encounters between people with mental illness and the police. The Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 28(3), 332-337.

Compton, M. T., Bahora, M., Watson, A. C., and Oliva, J. R. (2008). A comprehensive review of extant research on Crisis Intervention Team (CIT) programs. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law Online, 36(1), 47-55.

Compton, M. T., Neubert, B. N. D., Broussard, B., McGriff, J. A., Morgan, R., and Oliva, J. R. (2009). Use of force preferences and perceived effectiveness of actions among Crisis Intervention Team (CIT) police officers and non-CIT officers in an escalating psychiatric crisis involving a subject with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, sbp146.

Cordner, G. (2006). People with mental illness. Problem-oriented guides for police, Problem-Specific Guides Series (40).

Cunningham, P., McKenzie, K., and Taylor, E. F. (2006). The struggle to provide community-based care to low-income people with serious mental illnesses. Health Affairs, 25(3), 694-705.

Deane, M. W., Steadman, H. J., Borum, R., Veysey, B. M., and Morrissey. J. P. (1999). Emerging partnerships between mental health and law enforcement. Psychiatric Services, 50(1), 99-101.

Division of Public Safety Leadership. (2013). Roundtable on Police response to People with Mental Illness. Johns Hopkins University, School of Education, Division of Public Safety Leadership, March 19, 2013, Columbia, Maryland.

Engel, R. S., and Silver, E. (2001). Policing mentally disordered suspects: A reexamination of the criminalization hypothesis. Criminology, 39(2), 225-252.

Fakhoury, W., and Priebe, S. (2002). The process of deinstitutionalization: An international overview. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 15(2), 187-192.

Frontline. (2005). Deinstitutionalization: A Psychiatric “Titanic.” Retrieved from http://www.pbs.org/wgbh/pages/frontline/shows/asylums/special/excerpt.html.

Hanafi, S., Bahora, M., Demir, B. N., and Compton, M. T. (2008). Incorporating crisis intervention team (CIT) knowledge and skills into the daily work of police officers: A focus group study. Community Mental Health Journal, 44(6), 427-432.

James, R. (1990). What do police officers really want from the mental health system? Hospital and Community Psychiatry, 41(6), 663.

Lamb, H. R., Weinberger, L. E., and DeCuir, W. J. (2002). The police and mental health. Psychiatric Services, 53(10), 1266-1271.

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

Lord, V. B., Bjerregaard, B., Blevins, K. R., and Whisman, H. (2011). Factors influencing the responses of crisis intervention team–certified law enforcement officers. Police Quarterly, 14(4), 388-406.

Lurigio, A. J., and Swartz, J. A. (2000). Changing the contours of the criminal justice system to meet the needs of persons with serious mental illness. Criminal Justice, 3, 45-108.

Nagel, T. (2005). The problem of global justice. Philosophy and Public Affairs, 33(2), 113-147.

Novak, K. J., and Engel, R. S. (2005). Disentangling the influence of suspects’ demeanor and mental disorder on arrest. Policing: An International Journal of Police Strategies and Management, 28(3), 493-512.

Peternelj-Taylor, C. (2008). Criminalization of the mentally ill. Journal of Forensic Nursing, 4(4), 185-187.

Psarra, V., Sestrini, M., Santa, Z., Petsas, D., Gerontas, A., Garnetas, C., and Kontis, K. (2008). Greek police officers’ attitudes towards the mentally ill. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 31(1), 77-85.

Teplin, L. A. (2000). Keeping the peace: Police discretion and mentally ill persons. National Institute of Justice Journal, 244, 8-15.

Teplin, L. A., McClelland, G. M., Abram, K. M., and Weiner, D. A. (2005). Crime victimization in adults with severe mental illness: Comparison with the National Crime Victimization Survey. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62(8), 911-921.

Tucker, A. S., Van Hasselt, V. B., and Russell, S. A. (2008). Law enforcement response to the mentally ill: An evaluative review. Brief Treatment and Crisis Intervention, 8(3), 236.

Vermette, H. S., Pinals, D. A., and Appelbaum, P. S. (2005). Mental health training for law enforcement professionals. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law Online, 33(1), 42-46.

Watson, A. C., Corrigan, P. W., and Ottati, V. (2004). Police officers’ attitudes toward and decisions about persons with mental illness. Psychiatric Services, 55(1), 49-53.

Wells, W., and Schafer, J. A. (2006). Officer perceptions of police responses to persons with a mental illness. Policing: An International Journal of Police Strategies and Management, 29(4), 578-601.

A.4

MENTAL HEALTH IN LATIN AMERICA AND THE CARIBBEAN

Dévora Kestel, M.Sc.
Pan American Health Organization

“There is no health without mental health.” Despite this powerful statement, the mental health situation in Latin America and Caribbean countries (LAC) still lags behind where it should and could be. To analyze this situation from a public health perspective, we have selected a few indicators that facilitate the comparison among countries and sub-regions, and that at the same time provide sufficient information to adequately understand the current situation and appreciate potential opportunities from a regional perspective.

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

Although the general data available from LAC present a situation that could be viewed as dismal, it is important to highlight that there are many good examples in the region worth replicating. There are countries that have been reforming—a continuously ongoing process—their mental health system for decades; there are also regions or towns within countries that use their autonomy to move the mental health agenda forward, even when the national situation is not as advanced.

This brief article intends to highlight some of the most salient features of mental health systems in LAC.

Burden, Prevalence, and Treatment Gap

Recent studies of the global burden of disease show once again the importance of considering mental health as a public health issue (1). Depression is the eleventh cause of disability globally (before TB, diabetes, and lung cancer), and it ranks from third to seventh in the Americas region, depending on the different subregions being considered (2).

Noncommunicable diseases (NCDs) account for 54 percent of the total global health burden, and mental health and substance use disorders and are the biggest contributors to the NCD burden. In LAC, the Disability Adjusted Life Years due to neuropsychiatric disorders amount to 14 percent of the total amount (11 percent mental and behavioral disorders, 3 percent intentional injuries) (1).

Comparing the prevalence of these disorders with the available records of attendance to mental health services allows for the identification of a treatment gap. A treatment gap represents the percentage of people with severe mental disorders that do not receive treatment (3). At the global level, data from 2004 showed the extent of the treatment gap: 35.5 to 50.3 percent of serious cases did not receive any treatment within the prior year in developed countries, but the proportion of cases not receiving any treatment in developing countries was much higher: 76 to 85 percent. These figures clearly indicate how the problem of mental health services availability is not just of concern in developing countries (4).

A recent study of the treatment gap in LAC highlights that 73.5 percent of adults with severe and moderate affective disorders, anxiety disorders, and substance use disorders in the Americas do not receive treatment (47.2 percent in North America, and 77.9 percent in LAC). In the United States, the treatment gap for schizophrenia is 42.0 percent, and in LAC, 56.2 percent (5).

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

Resources Availability

One partial explanation for this significant gap is the inadequacy of funds available to develop appropriate services for those suffering from mental and neurological disorders. The world median of the health budget allocated to mental health is 2.82 percent (6).

Mental health expenditures at the regional level are not that different from global levels; the median health budget allocated to mental health is 2.3 percent, with differences linked to sub-regional characteristics.

In the context of the existing limited budget environment, it is very important to understand how those resources are used. The principal part of that budget goes to outdated, custodial style, psychiatric hospitals, with very limited funds made available for the development of community-based mental health services. Specifically, in the English-speaking Caribbean countries, the mental health budget is 3.5 percent of the health budget, and 84 percent of that budget goes to mental hospitals. In Central America, the mental health budget is even lower, at 1.5 percent of the health budget, with 75 percent of it spent in mental hospitals. In South America, the budget represents 2 percent of the health budget, with 66 percent going to mental hospitals (7).

Figure A-1 illustrates the median percentage of the government health budget allocated to mental health and to psychiatric hospitals, by subregion and total.

Policies, Plans, and Laws in Mental Health

Having a mental health policy and or a plan (whether it is independent or integrated as part of a general health document is not relevant) is very important to concentrate efforts and available resources into common objectives that will lead to a positive impact on the mental health of the population.

Most of the countries and territories in the region have developed mental health policies and plans; only six of them still do not have such a policy tool. However, having a document written and approved does not necessarily mean that it is being implemented. Several countries have a newly developed mental health policy or plan that advocates for the development of community-based mental health services, while, for example, their services remain concentrated in mental hospitals (7).

Regarding mental health legislation, only eight countries have legislation specific for mental health issued after the year 2000. The implications in this context are related to the lack of appropriate instruments to protect and promote the human rights of the mentally ill and their families.

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Image
FIGURE A-1 Mental health expenditures (7).
SOURCE: Figure developed by Dévora Kestel for the WHO-AIMS: Report on Mental Health Systems in Latin America and the Caribbean, by Dévora Kestel, copyright 2013 PAHO. Reprinted with the permission of the Pan American Health Organization.

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

Mental Health Professionals

Broadly speaking about human resources, there is limited availability of personnel working in mental health. At the global level, there is less than one psychiatrist per 200,000 people or more.

In the LAC region, there are 2.1 psychiatrists per 100,000 people. Although the national average in some countries may not necessarily be small, most of them are frequently concentrated in the capital or main cities of the country, leaving large territories uncovered. Other mental health professionals are generally less present in the region. The presence of nurses, psychologists (with the exception of some countries in South America), social workers, and occupational therapists is quite limited, ranging from around two to less than one professional of each category per 100,000 inhabitants.

Table A-1 below offers a summary of mental health workers available in the LAC region (7).

Organization of Mental Health Services

The analysis of existing services in LAC highlights the inefficiency in the location of beds. Globally, 62 percent of psychiatric beds are located in mental hospitals, with 21 percent in general hospitals, and just 16 percent in residential facilities (6, 7).

TABLE A-1 Mental Health Professionals in LAC

Subregion Psychiatrists Nurses Psychologists Social Workers Occupational Therapists Others
Central America, Mexico and Latin Caribbean 1.5 2.3 2 0.7 0.2 2.3
Non-Latin Caribbean 1.9 14.3* 0.3 1.1 0.1 20.8
South America 2.9 1.6 10.2 1.1 0.2 3.8
Total average 2.1 6 4.2 1 0.2 9

* A few islands with a small population and a relatively high number of general nurses (all involved with mental health patients) create this high number.

SOURCE: Presented by Dévora Kestel on October 30, 2015.

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

In LAC, 86.6 percent of the total number of available psychiatric beds are located in psychiatric hospitals, meaning that in many countries, the only answer for people suffering from mental disorders is a bed in a psychiatric hospital, which, in the majority of cases, are outdated custodial institutions that promote a systematic violation of human rights (7).

If a person is in need of hospitalization for an acute episode, he or she would have to access one of the 10.6 percent of the total number of available beds located in general hospitals that are dedicated to psychiatric care (7).

For those persons who may need a longer time to recover without necessarily needing hospital care, they should be considered lucky to access the limited number of beds available in residential facilities (2.7 percent of the total number of available beds) (7).

Paradoxically, when looking at the flow of patients in different mental health facilities, results indicate that those same hospitals that concentrate most of the limited available resources deal with between 5 and 13 percent of the total number of patients who visit any mental health facility in the year assessed. (7) The rest of the patients are seen by ambulatory services, in general hospitals, or in any other service available at the community level, developed with only 12 percent of the budget available for mental health (as mentioned above, 88 percent of the budget dedicated to mental health goes to traditional mental hospitals).

Table A-2 illustrates the number of mental health services’ users, by 100,000 people, visiting available facilities in the year of World Health Organization Assessment Instrument for Mental Health Systems (WHO-AIMS)

TABLE A-2 Number of Users Attending Mental Health Facilities

Subregion Outpatient Facilities (median) Day Hospitals Psychiatric Units in General Hospitals Residential Facilities Psychiatric Hospitals
South America 1.232 22.3 83.3 4.7 70.72
Central America, Mexico and Latin Caribbean 588 5.1 50 0.6 68
Non-Latin Caribbean 936 7.5 119 2.5 171.4

SOURCE: Presented by Dévora Kestel on October 30, 2015.

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

implementation in their respective country. Although the data collection is not precise (sometimes miscounting a patient with a contact), it nevertheless offers an idea of the patient flow. For instance, looking at South America, 70.72 patients were treated in psychiatric hospitals, while over a thousand were treated in all other services available and developed with very limited resources (7).

Good Experiences in the Region

In 1990, a regional Conference for Restructuring of Psychiatric Care was held in Caracas, Venezuela. That conference is considered to be an important milestone in the region because of its recognition of the need to move the attention, until then primarily focused on the psychiatric hospital, to the development of decentralized, community-based services that are participatory, comprehensive, and that ensure continuity of care. The conference also introduced the need to ensure the protection of patients’ human rights.

Since then, several regional declarations paired with Pan American Health Organization (PAHO) and WHO resolutions have been expanded and approved by member states, all of which aim at the development of a mental health system that will eventually provide an appropriate answer to the mental health needs of the population.

In parallel, several countries in the region are at different stages of serious reforms to their mental health systems, in some cases at the national level, and in others at the regional or even local level.

In October 2013, the World Health Assembly approved a Comprehensive Mental Health Plan of Action that emphasized the need to move ahead in this path that was initiated several years or decades ago (8).

In 2014, PAHO prepared an update to its Regional Mental Health Plan of Action to align it with the global plan. The regional plan is expected to be approved by the Ministers of Health of the Americas by October 2014.

The priorities identified in these plans are concentrated around four main areas:

  • Leadership and governance
  • Community-based mental health and social care services
  • Promotion and prevention
  • Information systems, evidence, and research

The adaptation of these plans to national realities should help countries move ahead with needed reforms to their existing mental health services.

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

Final Considerations

Although countries are moving toward the development of community-based mental health services that are decentralized and closer to people’s realities, there is still much to do to have the region ready to answer to situations related to violence or other specific needs, such as the appropriate response to populations affected by disasters (natural or man-made) and the needs of vulnerable groups.

Countries in the region will be ready to answer to these situations when an appropriate range of mental health services, from promotion and prevention to rehabilitation and recovery, based in the community will be available and when mental health will fully be integrated with general health services.

When discussing violence and mental health, mental health professionals should be aware that most LACs’ mental health systems create and direct violence toward individuals with mental disorders and their families. Until changes occur to their mental health systems, this violence will continue to neglect to provide patients with the attention and services they need and will maintain the idea that the mentally ill are violent.

Integrating mental health with general health services is a good strategy to ensure that the health system as a whole will offer adequate care to the people who need it.

References

1. Murray, C. J. L. et al. Disability-adjusted life years (DALYs) for 291 diseases and injuries in 21 regions, 1990–2010: A systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study. 2010. Lancet, 2012; 380: 2197–2223.

2. Ferrari, A. J., Charlson, F. J., Norman, R. E., Patten, S. B., Freedman G., et al. 2013. Burden of depressive disorders by country, sex, age and year: Findings from the Global Burden of Disease Study 2010. PLoS Med 10(11): e1001547. doi: 10.1371/journal. pmed.1001547.

3. Kohn, R., Saxena, S., Levav, I., and Saraceno, B. 2004. The treatment gap in mental health care. Bull World Health Organ 82: 858-66 pmid: 15640922.

4. World Health Organization. World Mental Health Survey Consortium. 2004. Prevalence, severity, and unmet need for treatment of mental disorders in the World Health Organization World Mental Health Surveys. JAVA, http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15173149.

5. Kohn, R. 2013. Treatment gap in the Americas, http://www.paho.org/hq/index.php?option=com_contentandview=articleandid=9408andItemid=99999.

6. World Health Organization. 2011. Mental Health ATLAS 2011, http://www.who.int/mental_health/publications/mental_health_atlas_2011/en.

7. Pan American Health Organization. 2013. WHO-AIMS: Report on mental health systems in Latin America and the Caribbean, http://www.paho.org/hq/index.php?option=com_contentandview=articleandid=935andItemid=1106andlang=enandlimitstart=7.

8. World Health Organization. Comprehensive mental health action plan 2013–2020, http://www.who.int/mental_health/publications/action_plan/en.

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

A.5

HEAVY EPISODIC ALCOHOL USE AND INTIMATE PARTNER VIOLENCE: A CROSS-CULTURAL PUBLIC HEALTH ISSUE

Cory A. Crane, Ph.D.
Kenneth E. Leonard, Ph.D.

Research Institute on Addictions
University at Buffalo

Intimate partner violence (IPV) involves the perpetration of physically aggressive acts against a spouse or dating partner and has been identified as a serious social concern with considerable health and financial costs at the individual, family, and societal levels (Lawrence et al., 2012). A review of 249 peer-reviewed articles detected a high prevalence of past year and lifetime (19.2 percent and 33.6 percent, respectively) physical IPV victimization among heterosexual individuals, with slightly higher rates of victimization among females (23.1 percent) than males (19.1 percent) across all studies (Desmarais et al., 2012). In a separate review of 111 articles, Desmarais and colleagues (2012) reported comparable rates of physical IPV perpetration across all participants (24.8 percent) with slightly higher rates of female-to-male (28.3 percent) than male-to-female (21.6 percent) perpetration.

Traditional conceptual models of IPV focused on constructs such as gender roles, power and control, and cultural sanctions for male-to-female aggression, and were advanced through a societal mandate to help explain and prevent IPV (Pence and Paymar, 1993). These early models spawned intervention approaches and public policies that focused on accountability as well as psychoeducation and remain predominant today (Babcock et al., 2004). Those who adhered to this early conceptual framework argued that alcohol use should be considered an unacceptable excuse for violent behavior and unequivocally rejected the notion that intoxication may play a causal role in episodes of IPV. The past 20 years of research, however, has largely failed to support both the traditional models (Dutton and Corvo, 2007) and associated interventions (Babcock et al., 2004), and has offered largely consistent empirical support for an alternative conceptualization of heavy alcohol use as a contributing causal factor in episodes of IPV (Leonard, 2005).

This research has been guided by and has contributed to the current prevailing theories describing the proximal effects of heavy alcohol consumption on violent behavior, which focus on attention allocation and the

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

disinhibiting characteristics of alcohol. These theories are based on the direct psychopharmacological effects of alcohol, which include a wide range of transitory impairments in higher-order executive cognitive functioning (Giancola et al., 2010). Among other effects, alcohol intoxication is thought to impair decision making through the exacerbation or amplification of attention to dominant environmental cues that may instigate violence while limiting attention to less salient cues that may inhibit violent responding, such as the long-term consequences of violent actions (Steele and Josephs, 1990; Taylor and Leonard, 1983). Alcohol expectancies serve as the basis for an alternative set of theories by which alcohol facilitates IPV through the perpetrator’s own beliefs about the mitigating effects of intoxication on one’s own culpability for socially unacceptable partner violent behavior. Although alcohol expectancy theories have received some scientific support, the majority of research has found little evidence for moderating effects on the relationship between alcohol intoxication and the perpetration of partner violence (e.g., Quigley and Leonard, 2006).

Human research contributing to our current understanding of the relationship between heavy alcohol use and IPV has utilized survey, longitudinal, event-based, experimental, and treatment-evaluation methodologies to support an assertion of causality based on the preponderance of evidence drawn from the multi-method approach (Leonard, 2005). Prior reviews have detected a significant effect of alcohol on general physical aggression (e.g., d = .22; Lipsey et al., 1997) and provided further empirical impetus to extend this work into the field of partner violence, resulting in multiple meta-analytic investigations examining the relationship between indices of heavy alcohol use and IPV within cross-sectional and case control studies, inclusive of considerable diversity in sample characteristics. These reviews find high heterogeneity across studies and significant overall effect sizes in the small to medium range. Ferrer and colleagues (2004) examined 9 studies and reported that male IPV perpetrators were more likely to abuse or be dependent upon alcohol than non-violent male participants (d = .57). Stith and colleagues (2004) reached similar conclusions regarding the effect size of alcohol on IPV perpetration after aggregating data from 22 studies of male participants (d = .48).

In an effort to update prior reviews and examine moderators that may account for the heterogeneity in effect sizes across studies reported in previous meta-analyses, Foran and O’Leary (2008) conducted the most recent and comprehensive review of this literature. In addition to corroborating the small-to-medium relationship between alcohol and IPV among males (d = .47), a small but tentatively homogeneous relationship was detected among eight studies involving female participants (d = .28). Moderation analyses examined the robustness of effects across various male sample characteristics. Effects did not differ across married and dating samples.

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

The relationship between alcohol and IPV was comparable across clinical and community samples as well. Not included in moderation analyses, the small-to-medium relationship between heavy alcohol use and IPV has been reported within incarcerated (e.g., Logan et al., 2001; White et al., 2001), emergency department (Lipsky et al., 2005), and military (Pan et al., 1994) samples, and nascent literature indicates an association among gay and lesbian samples, as well (Klostermann et al., 2011). Furthermore, research indicates that the link between heavy alcohol use and IPV is a cross-cultural phenomenon extending across ethnic groups (Caetano et al., 2001) and beyond Western nations with varying alcohol use and violence norms, including countries in South America (Kishor and Johnson, 2004), Asia (Rao, 1997), and Africa (Yigzaw et al., 2005). Most recently, problematic male alcohol use was determined to increase the odds of male-to-female IPV perpetration in 12 of 14 regions (N = 24,097 across 10 countries) that participated in the World Health Organization Multi-Country Study on Women’s Health and Domestic Violence population-based survey study between the years of 2000 and 2003 (Abramsky et al., 2011). Thus, the existing literature provides consistent evidence for an association between heavy alcohol use and IPV that can be observed widely.

There have been fewer longitudinal investigations addressing the association between heavy alcohol use and IPV. However, the available evidence suggests that heavy alcohol use is longitudinally predictive of IPV perpetration over short follow-up periods, but the evidence is less supportive over long follow-up periods. Although not a longitudinal study, Leonard et al. (1985) found that alcohol disorders within the past 3 years were predictive of husband IPV, while alcohol disorders that occurred before the last 3 years were not. This observation is consistent with longitudinal research involving premarital (Heyman et al., 1995) and newlywed (Quigley and Leonard, 1999) couples in which alcohol problems were predictive of IPV at the earliest, but not subsequent, follow-ups. Leonard and Senchack (1996) reported that prospective reports of premarital problematic alcohol use were predictive of physical IPV perpetration among husbands during the subsequent first year of marriage, even after controlling for premarital IPV. Similarly, Keller and colleagues (2009) reported that husband, but not wife, alcohol problems predicted increased physical IPV at a 2-year follow-up. The composite longitudinal evidence indicates that problematic alcohol use may be longitudinally associated with subsequent IPV and that this relationship may attenuate over time, possibly in response to shifting patterns of alcohol use as well as dynamic dyadic coping and adjustment strategies.

These studies demonstrate a general association between alcohol and IPV, but lack the temporal element necessary to establish causation. This proximal relationship can be best established through event-based as well as

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

laboratory analogue or experimental methodologies. Event-based research reveals that alcohol is often involved in IPV events and that perpetrator intoxication is predictive of both the occurrence and severity of violence. On the international scale, a cross-sectional study of the most severe episodes of physical IPV experienced by male and female respondents in 13 nations revealed relative uniformity in greater violence severity among alcohol-involved events compared to sober events (Graham et al., 2011). In the United States, an examination of epidemiological data provided by a subset of male (n = 501) and female (n = 1,756) participants who reported IPV victimization on the National Violence Against Women Survey (NVAWS; Tjaden and Thoennes, 2000) demonstrated that 33.6 percent of IPV events involved perpetrators under the influence of alcohol and that, after controlling for victim alcohol use, females were more likely to sustain injuries if their male partner had been drinking heavily (Thompson and Kingree, 2006).

Past year conflict interviews of 366 newlywed couples were conducted at their first wedding anniversary and similarly revealed that husbands were more likely to be drinking during physically, rather than verbally, violent conflicts (Leonard and Quigley, 1999). Participants from 61 of these couples completed conflict interviews after the third anniversary and described both an alcohol-involved and a non-alcohol-involved IPV episode (Testa et al., 2003). Episodes of IPV in which the husband reported alcohol use evidenced more violent acts, more severe violence, and greater mutual violence than episodes that did not involve husband alcohol use. High rates of perpetrator alcohol use during IPV events have also been reported by victims in emergency departments (Lipsky et al., 2005) and higher blood alcohol levels have been reported during violent conflicts in comparison to nonviolent conflicts by treatment-seeking alcoholics (Murphy et al., 2005). At the greatest extreme, proxy and victim reports reveal that rates of problematic drinking may be even higher among intimate partner homicide perpetrators (52.0 percent) than nonfatal partner violence perpetrators (30.9 percent) and that the majority of intimate partner homicides (59.5 percent) may occur while the perpetrator is under the influence of alcohol (Campbell et al., 2003; Sharps et al., 2001).

Prospective research techniques (e.g., daily diary and ecological momentary assessment) represent an amalgam of longitudinal and event-based methodologies and allow for a daily process analysis of the relationship between heavy alcohol use and IPV through frequent reporting. A single, recent investigation collected 56 independent, daily reports from both members of 118 community couples (Testa and Derrick, 2014). Despite detecting expectedly few acts of IPV during the study period, perpetrator alcohol use significantly predicted subsequent physical IPV perpetration both later in the same day and within 4 hours.

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

These types of observational designs provide strong evidence for correlations between heavy alcohol use and IPV but retrospective, self-reported accounts of alcohol-involved IPV are subject to recall errors and other reporting biases. Although previous meta-analyses have intentionally excluded the experimental literature (e.g., Foran and O’Leary, 2008), a number of carefully controlled experimental studies have examined the direct effects of alcohol administration on proxy measures of IPV and remain integral to establishing the case for causality under the most rigorous criteria. The sexual (for a review, see Abbey, 2011; Rehm et al., 2012) and general (for a review, see Exum, 2006) aggression literatures are replete with examples of experimental evidence supporting the effects of heavy alcohol consumption on aggressive and violent responding, such as setting more frequent or intense shocks for an opponent after having received alcohol in a competitive reaction time task (Bushman, 1997). The ecological validity of traditional aggression paradigms to IPV perpetration has been called into question, necessitating the generation of contemporary assessment methodologies that more closely approximate relationship-specific stressors, instigators, and aggressive response options (Tedeschi and Quigley, 1996).

Conflict resolution studies recruit couples to identify and discuss sources of relationship conflict with active alcohol manipulations. Coding of these recorded conversations has revealed effects of alcohol on IPV-related verbal behaviors. In a sample of 145 newlywed couples, Leonard and Roberts (1998) reported that husbands who had received alcohol, as well as their sober wives, displayed greater verbal negativity than those couples in which the husband had received either a placebo or nonalcoholic beverage. Similar effects of acute alcohol consumption on negativity during couple interactions have been detected among alcohol-abusing participants (Haber and Jacob, 1997; Jacob and Krahn, 1988; Jacob and Leonard, 1988). Other research has demonstrated that the effects of alcohol on negativity among alcohol abusing husbands may be confined to those with high antisocial tendencies (Jacob et al., 2001). A recent study (Testa et al., 2014) offered discrepant findings, detecting no negative effects of a couple’s alcohol manipulation on relationship interactions. The couples in this study were unique, however, in that they reported relatively high relationship satisfaction, were involved in stable long-term relationships, and were both heavy social drinkers, suggesting that they had likely developed satisfactory skills to cope with alcohol-involved conflict.

An alternative experimental paradigm involves recoding verbal responses to audio situations involving a simulated intimate partner. Examination of response content for aggressive intent consistently reveals that participants react with greater aggression following alcohol administration and anger induction. Eckhardt (2007) randomly assigned a sample of 102 husbands to an alcohol, placebo, or nonalcoholic beverage condition and

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

reported the greatest aggressive verbalizations among the participants who received alcohol, particularly if they had reported previously perpetrating marital violence. Using the same paradigm, Eckhardt and Crane (2008) detected a comparable effect of alcohol on aggressive verbalizations among a sample of 37 male and 33 female participants, particularly among those who reported high dispositional aggressive responding.

The research detailed above strongly supports alcohol as a contributing cause to IPV. Heavy or problematic alcohol use at the time of IPV perpetration does not function as a unilateral determinant of IPV, however, as evidenced by small-to-medium effect sizes and high heterogeneity across meta-analyses, IPV episodes that involve no alcohol in event-based studies, as well as variability across individuals in experimental data. Emerging from the aforementioned disinhibition and attention allocation models, individual and situational factors may increase the likelihood of alcohol-involved IPV. Indeed, individuals with low relationship satisfaction (e.g., Leonard and Senchak, 1996), high dispositional anger (e.g., Norlander and Eckhardt, 2005), aggressive tendencies (e.g., Eckhardt and Crane, 2008), and antisociality (e.g., Leonard and Senchak, 1996) seem to be at high risk of IPV perpetration following heavy alcohol consumption. Similarly, longitudinal data suggest that alcohol problems are associated with increased IPV perpetration the following year only among husbands high in both hostility and avoidance coping strategies (Schumacher et al., 2008). However, as noted by Leonard (2005), there is evidence to suggest that this synergistic effect breaks down among the most highly aggressive, and that for these individuals, alcohol may increase the severity of aggression, but not the occurrence. This notion, which came to be referred to as the “Multiple Thresholds” theory, has been explicated by Fals-Stewart, Leonard, and Birchler (2005).

Treatment

It is important to recognize that alcohol is neither a necessary nor a sufficient cause of intimate partner violence but instead, it contributes, in concert with other factors, to an increase in the occurrence and severity of such violence. Hence, in some groups, alcohol may have a minimal impact on IPV, while in other groups, its impact may be quite substantial. This is most apparent when we examine treatment populations. These populations offer a unique opportunity to observe both the association between problematic alcohol use and IPV as well as mutual changes in these conditions over time. We see strikingly high rates of comorbidity regardless of the identified treatment sample. Alcohol problems are detected in upwards of 50 percent of mandated and voluntary male IPV treatment seekers (e.g., Brown et al., 1999; Dalton, 2001; Gondolf, 1999; Stuart, et al., 2003).

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

High rates of hazardous drinking are also detected among female IPV offenders (Stuart et al., 2003). Rates of IPV among substance abuse treatment seeking samples also routinely exceed 50 percent in both male and female samples (e.g., Chase et al., 2003; Gondolf and Foster, 1991; Murphy and O’Farrell, 1994; Murphy et al., 2001).

Despite the high co-occurrence of problematic alcohol use and IPV, the behaviors are not routinely assessed unless included among the initial referral questions (Easton et al., 2007). Little evidence supports the effectiveness of IPV treatment programs at preventing subsequent acts of violence. Meta-analytic reviews have revealed small or non-significant effects, suggesting that treatment may reduce the risk of IPV by as little as 5 percent beyond legal intervention alone (e.g., Babcock et al., 2004; Feder and Wilson, 2005). Eckhardt and colleagues (2013) recently collected all case controlled studies of IPV treatment programs to report that the existing research, though methodologically flawed, contained roughly equivocal support for and against the effectiveness of IPV interventions. The failure of IPV treatment programs has been attributed, in part, to poor rates of attendance and high attrition (Babcock and Steiner, 1999; Gondolf, 2000). Heavy alcohol use may also impact the poor outcomes of IPV treatment inasmuch as IPV perpetrators with alcohol problems attend fewer sessions and drop out of treatment at a greater rate than perpetrators without drinking problems (for a review, see Daly and Pelowski, 2000; Olver et al., 2011).

Indeed, substance abuse treatment success has been associated with reductions in IPV. One investigation found that intensive treatment for alcohol dependence, in the absence of IPV-specific content, resulted in significant reductions not only in alcohol use but also physical and psychological IPV perpetration at 6- and 12-month follow-ups, according to male clients and their wives (Stuart et al., 2003). Similar results were found among a sample of females seeking treatment for alcohol dependence; treatment resulted in reductions in alcohol, physical violence, and psychological IPV perpetration at 6- and 12-month follow ups (Stuart et al., 2002). In a larger treatment evaluation study, alcohol treatment resulted in reductions in the prevalence of IPV from baseline assessment to 1-year follow-up and a greater increase in IPV among relapsed clients relative to remitted clients at 2 year follow-up (O’Farrell et al., 2003).

When detected, however, dual alcohol problems and IPV most often result in assignment to separate treatment programs that fail to coordinate efforts to minimize client burden, reducing the likelihood of completing either program (Bennett and Lawson, 1994; Schumacher et al., 2003). Initial evidence suggests that integrated alcohol and IPV treatments may be more effective than treatment as usual. Evaluations of behavioral couples therapy for alcohol and IPV problems have evidenced reductions in alcohol use as well as the prevalence of IPV among both male and female partners

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

at a 1-year follow-up, again with less violence among remitted compared to relapsed clients (O’Farrell et al., 2000; O’Farrell et al., 2004). A separate investigation reported significant reductions in IPV from baseline to post-treatment among clients randomly assigned to an integrated cognitive-behavioral therapy for substance abuse and IPV but not clients assigned to an alternative, standard treatment protocol (Easton et al., 2007).

Thus, research among treatment-seeking samples offers further confirmation of the general interrelationship between alcohol and IPV while highlighting the need to address both conditions among relevant clinical samples. Problematic alcohol use is associated with IPV treatment dropout, and successful substance abuse treatment reduces the risk of future IPV perpetration beyond violence interventions alone. Even still, integrated programs for clients with special treatment needs are routinely met with resistance and remain more the exception than the norm.

Future Directions

Although we have briefly reviewed the literature that has contributed to our considerable understanding of the effects of acute heavy alcohol consumption on IPV, there is a dearth of research into the effects of chronic heavy alcohol use on the risk of perpetrating IPV. The processes by which chronic heavy alcohol consumption may affect the ability to interpret and select prosocial responses to incoming social stimuli remain unclear. One model describes indirect effects, positing that chronic alcohol use increases the risk of IPV through gradual changes to interpersonal dynamics that reduce relationship satisfaction, increase relationship stress, and decrease reliance upon non-violent conflict resolution tactics designed to accommodate a partner or improve the partnership (Quigley and Leonard, 1999). Alternatively, neurobiological evidence reveals that chronic alcohol use is associated with neuronal death and widespread deficits in executive cognitive functioning (e.g., Sullivan et al., 2002). Easton and colleagues (2008) extended this research to report greater cognitive impairment (e.g., attention, concentration, flexibility) among alcohol-dependent men who reported IPV perpetration when compared to alcohol-dependent men who reported no IPV perpetration. As previously stated, much of the existing research provides evidence for the proximal effects of heavy alcohol consumption on IPV, even among individuals with chronic alcohol abuse problems. Additional long-term longitudinal and neuropsychological research beginning in adolescence and extending through early adulthood is required to further develop our understanding of the biopsychosocial effects of chronic alcohol use on IPV.

The role of psychopathology, as well as its interaction with alcohol, in IPV perpetration represents another underdeveloped area of research. Similar to alcohol, early IPV models conceptualized mental illness as an excuse

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

for unacceptable violence (Pence and Paymar, 1993). Although emerging research now suggests that certain mental health conditions associated with impulse control problems or affective dysregulation, such as mood and anxiety disorders, may increase the risk of IPV perpetration (Crane et al., 2014; Hatters-Friedman and Loue, 2007), existing IPV treatment protocols fail to take mental illness into consideration. Given initial evidence that suggests a synergistic effect of substance use and mental illness on violent behavior (e.g., Van Dorn et al., 2012), the effects of mental illness on compliance and recidivism may need to be evaluated within the context of IPV treatment and existing interventions adapted to improve outcomes for this select group as well as their partners.

Our review of IPV treatment research suggests that current options are insufficient to accomplish the goal of violence prevention and that there is a great need for the development and evaluation of more effective interventions that focus on individual needs rather than a uniform protocol. Heavy alcohol consumption, as a contributing cause of IPV, and interactions between alcohol use and partner influences are emerging as important treatment considerations. As such, nascent research into integrated treatment programs for both problematic alcohol use and IPV show promise, as do relationship-systems interventions that focus on dynamic, interactive processes at the couple level. The available treatment portfolio and public policies that govern their implementation must be expanded to accommodate individual substance use, psychiatric, and dyadic needs.

References

Abbey, A. (2011). Alcohol’s role in sexual violence perpetration: Theoretical explanations, existing evidence and future directions. Drug and Alcohol Review, 30, 481-489.

Abramsky, T., Watts, C. H., Garcia-Moreno, C., Devries, K., Kiss, L., Ellsberg, M., Hansen, H. A., and Heise, L. (2011). What factors are associated with recent intimate partner violence? Findings from the WHO multi-country study on women’s health and domestic violence. BMC Public Health, 11, 109-125.

Babcock, J. C., Green, C. E., and Robie, C. (2004). Does batterers’ treatment work? A meta-analytic review of domestic violence treatment. Clinical Psychology Review, 23, 1023-1053.

Babcock, J. C., and Steiner, R. (1999). The relationship between treatment, incarceration, and recidivism of battering: A program evaluation of Seattle’s coordinated community response to domestic violence. Journal of Family Psychology, 13, 46-59.

Bennett, L., and Lawson, M. (1994). Barriers to cooperation between domestic-violence and substance-abuse programs. Families in Society, 75, 277-286.

Brown, T. G., Werk, A., Caplan, T., and Seraganian, P. (1999). Violent substance abusers in domestic violence treatment. Violence and Victims, 14, 179-190.

Bushman, B. J. (1997). Effects of alcohol on human aggression. Validity of proposed explanations. Recent Developments in Alcoholism, 13, 227-243.

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

Caetano, R., Schafer, J., and Cunradi, C. B. (2001). Alcohol-related intimate partner violence among White, Black, and Hispanic couples in the United States. Alcohol Research and Health, 25, 58-65.

Campbell, J. C., Webster, D., Koziol-McLain, J., Block, C., Campbell, D., Curry, M. A., … and Laughon, K. (2003). Risk factors for femicide in abusive relationships: Results from a multisite case control study. American Journal of Public Health, 93, 1089-1097.

Chase, K. A., O’Farrell, T. J., Murphy, C. M., Fals-Stewart, W., and Murphy, M. (2003). Factors associated with partner violence among female alcoholic patients and their male partners. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 64, 137-149.

Crane, C. A., Hawes, S. W., Devine, S., and Easton, C. J. (2014). Axis I psychopathology and the perpetration of intimate partner violence. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 70, 238-247.

Dalton, B. (2001). Batterer characteristics and treatment completion. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 16, 1223-1238.

Daly, J. E., and Pelowski, S. (2000). Predictors of dropout among men who batter: A review of studies with implications for research and practice. Violence and Victims, 15, 137-160.

Desmarais, S. L., Reeves, K. A., Nicholls, T. L., Telford, R. P., and Fiebert, M. S. (2012). Prevalence of physical violence in intimate relationships, part 2: Rates of male and female perpetration. Partner Abuse, 3, 170-198.

Dutton, D. G., and Corvo, K. (2007). The Duluth Model: A data-impervious paradigm and a failed strategy. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 12, 658-667.

Easton, C. J., Mandel, D. L., Hunkele, K. A., Nich, C., Rounsaville, B. J., and Carroll, K. M. (2007). A cognitive behavioral therapy for alcohol-dependent domestic violence offenders: An integrated Substance Abuse–Domestic Violence treatment approach (SADV). The American Journal on Addictions, 16, 24-31.

Easton, C. J., Sacco, K. A., Neavins, T. M., Wupperman, P., and George, T. P. (2008). Neurocognitive performance among alcohol dependent men with and without physical violence toward their partners: A preliminary report. The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 34, 29-37.

Eckhardt, C. I. (2007). Effects of alcohol intoxication on anger experience and expression among partner assaultive men. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75, 61-71.

Eckhardt, C. I., and Crane, C. (2008). Effects of alcohol intoxication and aggressivity on aggressive verbalizations during anger arousal. Aggressive Behavior, 34, 428-436.

Eckhardt, C. I., Murphy, C. M., Whitaker, D. J., Sprunger, J., Dykstra, R., and Woodard, K. (2013). The effectiveness of intervention programs for perpetrators and victims of intimate partner violence. Partner Abuse, 4, 196-231.

Exum, M. L. (2006). Alcohol and aggression: An integration of findings from experimental studies. Journal of Criminal Justice, 34, 131-145.

Fals-Stewart, W., Leonard, K. E., and Birchler, G. R. (2005). The occurrence of male-to-female intimate partner violence on days of men’s drinking: The moderating effects of antisocial personality disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 239-248.

Feder, L., and Wilson, D. B. (2005). A meta-analytic review of court-mandated batterer intervention programs: Can courts affect abusers’ behavior? Journal of Experimental Criminology, 1, 239-262.

Ferrer, V., Bosch, E., Garcia, E., Manassero, M. A., and Gili, M. (2004). Meta-analytic study of differential characteristics between batterers and non-batterers: The case of psychopathology and consumption of alcohol and drugs. Psykhe, 13, 141-156.

Foran, H. M., and O’Leary, K. D. (2008). Alcohol and intimate partner violence: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 1222-1234.

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

Giancola, P. R., Josephs, R. A., Parrott, D. J., and Duke, A. A. (2010). Alcohol myopia revisited clarifying aggression and other acts of disinhibition through a distorted lens. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 265-278.

Gondolf, E. W. (1999). A comparison of four batterer intervention systems: Do court referral, program length, and services matter? Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 14, 41-61.

Gondolf, E. W. (2000). How batterer program participants avoid reassault. Violence against Women, 6, 1204-1222.

Gondolf, E. W., and Foster, R. A. (1991). Wife assault among VA alcohol rehabilitation patients. Psychiatric Services, 42, 74-79.

Graham, K., Bernards, S., Wilsnack, S. C., and Gmel, G. (2011). Alcohol may not cause partner violence but it seems to make it worse: A cross national comparison of the relationship between alcohol and severity of partner violence. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 26, 1503-1523.

Haber, J. R., and Jacob, T. (1997). Marital interactions of male versus female alcoholics. Family Process, 36, 385-402.

Hatters-Friedman, S. H., and Loue, S. (2007). Incidence and prevalence of intimate partner violence by and against women with severe mental illness. Journal of Women’s Health, 16, 471-480.

Heyman, R. E., O’Leary, K. D., and Jouriles, E. N. (1995). Alcohol and aggressive personality styles: Potentiators of serious physical aggression against wives? Journal of Family Psychology, 9, 44.

Jacob, T., and Krahn, G. L. (1988). Marital interactions of alcoholic couples: Comparison with depressed and nondistressed couples. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 73-79.

Jacob, T., and Leonard, K. E. (1988). Alcoholic-spouse interaction as a function of alcoholism subtype and alcohol consumption interaction. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97, 231-237.

Jacob, T., Leonard, K. E., and Randolph Haber, J. (2001). Family interactions of alcoholics as related to alcoholism type and drinking condition. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 25, 835-843.

Keller, P. S., El-Sheikh, M., Keiley, M., and Liao, P. (2009). Longitudinal relations between marital aggression and alcohol problems. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 23, 2-13.

Kishor, S., and Johnson, K. (2004). Profiling domestic violence: A multi-country study. Calverton, MD: ORC Macro.

Klostermann, K., Kelley, M. L., Milletich, R. J., and Mignone, T. (2011). Alcoholism and partner aggression among gay and lesbian couples. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 16, 115-119.

Lawrence, E., Orengo-Aguayo, R., Langer, A., and Brock, R. L. (2012). The impact and consequences of partner abuse on partners. Partner Abuse, 3, 406-428.

Leonard, K. E. (2005). Alcohol and intimate partner violence: When can we say that heavy drinking is a contributing cause of violence? Addiction, 100, 422-425.

Leonard, K. E., Bromet, E. J., Parkinson, D. K., Day, N. L., and Ryan, C. M. (1985). Patterns of alcohol use and physically aggressive behavior in men. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 46, 279-282.

Leonard, K. E., and Quigley, B. M. (1999). Drinking and marital aggression in newlyweds: An event-based analysis of drinking and the occurrence of husband marital aggression. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 60, 537-545.

Leonard, K. E., and Roberts, L. J. (1998). The effects of alcohol on the marital interactions of aggressive and nonaggressive husbands and their wives. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 602-615.

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

Leonard, K. E., and Senchak, M. (1996). Prospective prediction of husband marital aggression within newlywed couples. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105, 369-380.

Lipsey, M. W., Wilson, D. B., Cohen, M. A., and Derzon, J. H. (1997). Is there a causal relationship between alcohol use and violence? In M. Galanter (Ed.), Recent developments in alcoholism: Vol. 13. Alcohol and violence – Epidemiology, neurobiology, psychology, family issues (First ed., pp. 245-282). Springer: US.

Lipsky, S., Caetano, R., Field, C. A., and Larkin, G. L. (2005). Psychosocial and substance-use risk factors for intimate partner violence. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 78, 39-47.

Logan, T. K., Walker, R., Staton, M., and Leukefeld, C. (2001). Substance use and intimate violence among incarcerated males. Journal of Family Violence, 16, 93-114.

Murphy, C. M., and O’Farrell, T. J. (1994). Factors associated with marital aggression in male alcoholics. Journal of Family Psychology, 8, 321-335.

Murphy, C. M., O’Farrell, T. J., Fals-Stewart, W., and Feehan, M. (2001). Correlates of intimate partner violence among male alcoholic patients. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 69, 528-540.

Murphy, C. M., Winters, J., O’Farrell, T. J., Fals-Stewart, W., and Murphy, M. (2005). Alcohol consumption and intimate partner violence by alcoholic men: Comparing violent and nonviolent conflicts. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 19, 35-42.

Norlander, B., and Eckhardt, C. (2005). Anger, hostility, and male perpetrators of intimate partner violence: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 25, 119-152.

O’Farrell, T. J., Fals-Stewart, W., Murphy, M., and Murphy, C. M. (2003). Partner violence before and after individually based alcoholism treatment for male alcoholic patients. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71, 92-102.

O’Farrell, T. J., Murphy, C. M., Neavins, T. M., and Van Hutton, V. (2000). Verbal aggression among male alcoholic patients and their wives in the year before and two years after alcoholism treatment. Journal of Family Violence, 15, 295-310.

O’Farrell, T. J., Murphy, C. M., Stephan, S. H., Fals-Stewart, W., and Murphy, M. (2004). Partner violence before and after couples-based alcoholism treatment for male alcoholic patients: The role of treatment involvement and abstinence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72, 202-217.

Olver, M. E., Stockdale, K. C., and Wormith, J. S. (2011). A meta-analysis of predictors of offender treatment attrition and its relationship to recidivism. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 79, 6-21.

Pan, H. S., Neidig, P. H., and O’Leary, K. D. (1994). Predicting mild and severe husband-to-wife physical aggression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 975-981.

Pence, E., and Paymar, M. (1993). Education groups for men who batter: The Duluth Model. New York: Springer Publishing Company.

Quigley, B. M., and Leonard, K. E. (1999). Husband alcohol expectancies, drinking, and marital-conflict styles as predictors of severe marital violence among newlywed couples. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 13, 49-59.

Quigley, B. M., and Leonard, K. E. (2006). Alcohol expectancies and intoxicated aggression. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 11, 484-496.

Rao, V. (1997). Wife-beating in rural South India: A qualitative and econometric analysis. Social Science and Medicine, 44, 1169-1180.

Rehm, J., Shield, K. D., Joharchi, N., and Shuper, P. A. (2012). Alcohol consumption and the intention to engage in unprotected sex: Systematic review and meta-analysis of experimental studies. Addiction, 107, 51-59.

Schumacher, J. A., Fals-Stewart, W., and Leonard, K. E. (2003). Domestic violence treatment referrals for men seeking alcohol treatment. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 24, 279-283.

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

Schumacher, J. A., Homish, G. G., Leonard, K. E., Quigley, B. M., and Kearns-Bodkin, J. N. (2008). Longitudinal moderators of the relationship between excessive drinking and intimate partner violence in the early years of marriage. Journal of Family Psychology, 22, 894-904.

Sharps, W., Campbell, J., Campbell, D., Gary, F., and Webster, D. P. (2001). The role of alcohol use in intimate partner femicide. American Journal on Addictions, 10, 122-135.

Steele, C. M., and Josephs, R. A. (1990). Alcohol myopia: Its prized and dangerous effects. American Psychologist, 45, 921-933.

Stith, S. M., Smith, D. B., Penn, C. E., Ward, D. B., and Tritt, D. (2004). Intimate partner physical abuse perpetration and victimization risk factors: A meta-analytic review. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 10, 65-98.

Stuart, G. L., Moore, T. M., Kahler, C. W., and Ramsey, S. E. (2003). Substance abuse and relationship violence among men court-referred to batterers’ intervention programs. Substance Abuse, 24, 107-122.

Stuart, G. L., Moore, T. M., Ramsey, S. E., and Kahler, C. W. (2003). Relationship aggression and substance use among women court-referred to domestic violence intervention programs. Addictive Behaviors, 28, 1603-1610.

Stuart, G. L., Ramsey, S. E., Moore, T. M., Kahler, C. W., Farrell, L. E., Recupero, P. R., and Brown, R. A. (2002). Marital violence victimization and perpetration among women substance abusers: A descriptive study. Violence Against Women, 8, 934-952.

Stuart, G. L., Ramsey, S. E., Moore, T. M., Kahler, C. W., Farrell, L. E., Recupero, P. R., and Brown, R. A. (2003). Reductions in marital violence following treatment for alcohol dependence. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 18, 1113-1131.

Sullivan, E. V., Fama, R., Rosenbloom, M. J., and Pfefferbaum, A. (2002). A profile of neuropsychological deficits in alcoholic women. Neuropsychology, 16, 74-83.

Taylor, S. P., and Leonard, K. E. (1983). Alcohol and human physical aggression. Aggression: Theoretical and Empirical Reviews, 2, 77-101.

Tedeschi, J. T., and Quigley, B. M. (1996). Limitations of laboratory paradigms for studying aggression. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 1, 163-177.

Testa, M., Crane, C. A., Quigley, B. M., Levitt, A., and Leonard, K. E. (2014). Effects of administered alcohol on intimate partner interactions in a conflict resolution paradigm. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 75, 249-258.

Testa, M., and Derrick, J. L. (2014). A daily process examination of the temporal association between alcohol use and verbal and physical aggression in community couples. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 28, 127-138.

Testa, M., Quigley, B. M., and Leonard, K. E. (2003). Does alcohol make a difference? Within-participants comparison of incidents of partner violence. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 18, 735-743.

Thompson, M. P., and Kingree, J. B. (2006). The roles of victim and perpetrator alcohol use in intimate partner violence outcomes. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 21, 163-177.

Tjaden, P., and Thoennes, N. (2000). Prevalence and consequences of male-to-female and female-to-male intimate partner violence as measured by the National Violence Against Women Survey. Violence Against Women, 6, 142-161.

Van Dorn, R., Volavka, J., and Johnson, N. (2012). Mental disorder and violence: Is there a relationship beyond substance use? Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 47, 487-503.

White, R. J., Ackerman, R. J., and Caraveo, L. E. (2001). Self-identified alcohol abusers in a low-security federal prison: Characteristics and treatment implications. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 45, 214-227.

Yigzaw, T., Yibric, A., and Kebede, Y. (2005). Domestic violence around Gondar in northwest Ethiopia. Ethiopian Journal of Health Development, 18, 133-139.

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

A.6

PEER BULLYING AND MENTAL HEALTH

Dieter Wolke, Ph.D.

University of Warwick, Department of Psychology (Lifespan Health and Wellbeing Group) and Division of Mental Health and Well-being (Warwick Medical School), Coventry, UK

This paper considers the importance of bullying as a major risk factor for physical and mental health and adaptation to adult roles, including forming lasting relationships and integrating into work and being economically independent. Evidence is provided that bullying by peers either at school or at home by siblings has been mostly ignored by health professionals but should be considered as a significant risk factor and safeguarding issue. Policy suggestions are made to more effectively recognize and manage affected children.

Bullying

Bullying is the systematic abuse of power and is defined as aggressive behavior or intentional harm doing among peers that is carried out repeatedly, and involves an imbalance of power, either actual or perceived, between the victim and the bully.1 Bullying can take the form of direct bullying, which includes physical and verbal acts of aggression such as hitting, stealing, name calling, or indirect bullying, which is characterised by social exclusion and rumor spreading.2,3,4 Children can be involved in bullying as victims and as bullies, but also as bully/victims, a subgroup of victims who display bullying behavior.5,6 Recently there has been much interest in cyberbullying, which can be broadly defined as any bullying which is performed via electronic means, such as mobile phones or the Internet. Approximately 50 percent of children report having been bullied at some point in their lives, and 10 to 14 percent experience chronic bullying lasting for more than 6 months.7,8 Between 2 and 5 percent are bullies, and a similar number are bully/victims in childhood/adolescence.9 Rates of cyberbullying are substantially lower, around 4.5 percent for victims and 2.8 percent for perpetrators (bullies; bully/victims), with up to 90 percent also traditionally (face to face) bullied.10 Being bullied by peers is the most frequent form of abuse encountered by children, much higher than abuse by parents or other adult perpetrators.11

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

Bullying Is Not a Conduct Disorder

Bullying is found in all societies, including modern hunter–gatherer societies and ancient civilizations. It is considered to be an evolutionary adaptation and the purpose is to gain access to resources, secure survival, reduce stress, and allow for more mating opportunities.12 Indeed, it has been shown that bullies are highly motivated to gain high status, dominance, and the strategic behavior enables access to social success and romantic partners.13 Indeed, many bullies of both sexes are bi-strategic, employing both means of bullying but also acts of aggressive “prosocial” behavior to enhance their own position by being public and making the recipient dependent as they cannot reciprocate.14 Indeed, bullies (but not bully/victims or victims) have been shown to be strong, to have good social and emotional understanding, and to be highly popular.15 Thus, they are not conduct disordered. Indeed, unlike conduct disorder, bullies are found in all socioeconomic groups16 and ethnic groups.17 If bullying is about power rather than an individual disorder, it should be more frequent in contexts where there is little cohesion and less equality in terms of access to resources. Indeed, bullying prevalence increases with inequality in nations,18 classrooms,19 and even at home.20 Furthermore, it has been shown that interventions to reduce bullying have little success if the bully is highly popular, i.e., has social power and there is no incentive to discontinue socially enhancing behavior.21 In contrast, victims have been described as withdrawn, unassertive, easily emotionally upset, and as having poor emotional or social understanding;15,22 whereas bully/victims tend to be aggressive, easily angered, low on popularity, and frequently bullied by their siblings,23 i.e., most like conduct-disordered children.

Adverse Consequences of Being Bullied

Until fairly recently, most studies on the effects of bullying were cross-sectional or just included brief follow-up periods. Thus, pre-existing mental health problems may explain that the children were bullied, rather than bullying being the “cause” of the problems. Furthermore, most studies just investigated victimization rather than also bullying perpetration and few distinguished between victims and bully/victims. However, recent longitudinal studies into adolescence,24,25,26 early adulthood8,9,27,28 and even into late adulthood29 indicate that being the victim of bullying is associated with often severe mental health problems, including anxiety disorders, depression, self-harm and suicide, personality disorder,30 and psychotic symptoms that are long lasting and persist up to 40 years later! The use of genetically sensitive designs where mono-zygotic twins (genetically identical living in the same households) but discordant for experiences of bullying

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

were compared, showed that internalizing problems increased over time only in those who were bullied.31 Furthermore, longitudinal studies allow for the control of pre-existing mental illness, family factors from parenting, domestic violence to abuse, and social disadvantage.32 Thus, there is mounting evidence that being exposed to bullying as victim or bully/victim has unique adverse impact on mental health.33 Furthermore, most recent evidence indicates that being bullied leads to highly increased difficulties in economic behavior, losing or leaving jobs, and lower income and overall poorer quality of life29,32,34 (see Figure A-2).

The carefully controlled longitudinal studies paint a converging picture of the long-term effects of being bullied in childhood. Firstly, the effects of being bullied are found beyond other childhood adversity and adult abuse.9 In fact, when compared to the experience of having been placed into care in childhood, the effects of frequent bullying were as detrimental 40 years later!29 Secondly, there is a dose–effect relationship between being victimized by peers and outcomes in adolescence and adulthood. Those who were bullied more frequently,29 more severely (i.e., direct and indirectly bullied25) or more chronically bullied (i.e., over a longer period of time8) show more adverse outcome. Thirdly, even those who have escaped from

Image
FIGURE A-2 The impact of being bullied on functioning in adulthood.8,9,27,29,32
SOURCE: Figure developed by Dieter Wolke.
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

being previously bullied at school age still show an increased risk for their health, self-worth, and quality of life years later.24 Fourthly, where victims and bully/victims have been considered separately, bully/victims seem to show the poorest outcome ranging from mental health, to economic adaptation, social relationships to early parenthood.9,27,32,35 Fifthly, studies that did distinguish between bullies and bully/victims found no adverse effects of being a pure bully on adverse adult outcomes. This is consistent with a view that bullies are highly sophisticated social manipulators who show little empathy and are callous.36

Processes

There are a variety of potential routes by which being victimized may affect later life outcomes. Being bullied may alter physiological responses to stress,37 interact with a genetic vulnerability such as variation in the serotonin transporter (5-HTT) gene,38 or affect telomere length (aging) or the epigenome.39 Altered HPA-axis activity and altered cortisol responses may not only increase the risk for developing mental health problems40 but also increase susceptibility to illness by interfering with immune responses.41

A recent study found that bullied children may experience higher than normal chronic inflammation and associated health problems that can persist into adulthood.42 Blood tests for C-reactive protein (CRP), a marker of low-grade systemic inflammation in the body often associated with cardiovascular disease, metabolic syndrome, and psychological disorders, revealed that CRP levels in the blood of bullied children increased with the number of times they were bullied. Additional blood tests carried out on the children after they had reached 19 and 21 years old revealed that those who were bullied as children had CRP levels more than twice as high as bullies, whereas bullies had CRP levels lower than those who were neither bullies nor victims (see Figure A-3). Thus, bullying others appears to have a protective effect in reducing the general rise in chronic inflammation from childhood to early adulthood. This is consistent with studies showing lower inflammation for individuals with higher socioeconomic status43 and studies with non-human primates showing health benefits for those higher in the social hierarchy.44 The clear implication of these findings is that both ends of the continuum of social status in peer relationships are important for inflammation levels and health status.

Furthermore, experiences of threat by peers may alter cognitive responses to threatening situations45 or affect school performance. Both altered stress responses and altered social cognition (e.g., being hypervigilant to hostile cues46) and neuro-circuitry47 related to bullying exposure may affect social relationships with parents, friends, and co-workers. Finally, victimization, in particular of bully-victims, has been found to be associated

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Image
FIGURE A-3 Adjusted mean young adult CRP levels (mg/L) based on childhood/adolescent bullying status.
NOTE: These values are adjusted for baseline CRP levels as well as other CRP-related covariates. All analyses used robust standard errors to account for repeated observations.
SOURCE: Reprinted with permission from Copeland et al., 2014.

with poor concurrent academic achievement.48 In the UK alone, more than 16,000 young people aged 11–15 are estimated to be absent from state school with bullying as the main reason, and 78,000 are absent where bullying is one of the reasons given for absence.49 The risk of failure to complete high school or college in chronic victims or bully/victims increases the risk of poorer income and job performance.32

Summary and Implications

Childhood bullying has serious effects on health, leading to substantial costs for individuals, their families and society at large. In the United States, it has been estimated that preventing high school bullying results in lifetime cost benefits of more than $1.4 million per individual.50 Many bullied children suffer in silence, and are reluctant to tell their parents or teachers about their experiences, for fear of reprisals or shame.51 Up to 50 percent of children say they would rarely, or never, tell their parents, while between 35 percent and 60 percent would not tell their teacher.52

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

Considering this evidence of ill effects of being bullied and the fact that children will have spent much more time with their peers than their parents by the time they reach 18 years of age, it is more than surprising that childhood bullying is not at the forefront as a major public health concern.53 Children are hardly ever asked about their peer relationships by health professionals. This is because health professionals are poorly educated about bullying and find it difficult to raise the subject or deal with it.54 To prevent violence against oneself (e.g., self-harm) and reduce mental health problems, it is imperative to address bullying!

Key Messages

  • Childhood bullying is a significant risk factor leading to harmful physical, psychological, and social effects that can last a lifetime.
  • It affects children from all socioeconomic backgrounds and ethnic groups and requires universal intervention.
  • There is a need for greater awareness and responsiveness in primary and secondary health care as part of a communitywide, integrated approach to stemming the effects of childhood bullying.
  • Evidence-based guidance needs to be developed on how best to identify affected children in health care, provide support to children and their parents, and, where necessary, make referrals to appropriate agencies for associated physical and mental health problems.
  • Effective interventions that can be delivered in primary care to minimize the consequences of being bullied are needed. These may include innovative online interventions.55
  • New approaches are needed to channel the considerable leadership abilities and need for social recognition of bullies into socially acceptable and prosocial activities.

References

1. Olweus D. Bullying at school: What we know and what we can do. Wiley-Blackwell; 1993.

2. Bjorkqvist K, Lagerspetz KM, Kaukiainen A. Do girls manipulate and boys fight? Developmental trends in regard to direct and indirect aggression. Aggressive Behavior. 1992;18(2):117-127.

3. Wolke D, Woods S, Bloomfield L, Karstadt L. The association between direct and relational bullying and behaviour problems among primary school children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2000;41(8):989-1002.

4. Crick NR, Grotpeter JK. Children’s treatment by peers: Victims of relational and overt aggression. Development and Psychopathology. 1996;8(02):367-380.

5. Haynie DL, Nansel T, Eitel P, et al. Bullies, victims, and bully/victims: Distinct groups of at-risk youth. The Journal of Early Adolescence. 2001;21(1):29-49.

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

6. Boulton MJ, Smith PK. Bully/victim problems in middle-school children: Stability, self-perceived competence, peer perceptions and peer acceptance. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 1994;12(3):315-329.

7. Analitis F, Velderman M, Ravens-Sieberer U. Being bullied: Associated factors in children and adolescents 8 to 18 years old in 11 European countries. Pediatrics. 2009;123(2):569-577.

8. Wolke D, Lereya ST, Fisher HL, Lewis G, Zammit S. Bullying in elementary school and psychotic experiences at 18 years: A longitudinal, population-based cohort study. Psychological Medicine. 2013;FirstView:1-13.

9. Copeland WE, Wolke D, Angold A, Costello E. Adult psychiatric outcomes of bullying and being bullied by peers in childhood and adolescence. JAMA Psychiatry. 2013;70(4):419-426.

10. Olweus D. Cyberbullying: An overrated phenomenon? European Journal of Developmental Psychology. 2012/09/01 2012;9(5):520-538.

11. Radford L, Corral S, Bradley C, Fisher H. The prevalence and impact of child maltreatment and other types of victimization in the UK: Findings from a population survey of caregivers, children and young people and young adults. Child Abuse and Neglect. 2013.

12. Volk AA, Camilleri JA, Dane AV, Marini ZA. Is Adolescent Bullying an Evolutionary Adaptation? Aggressive Behavior. 2012;38:222-238.

13. Olthof T, Goossens FA, Vermande MM, Aleva EA, van der Meulen M. Bullying as strategic behavior: Relations with desired and acquired dominance in the peer group. Journal of School Psychology. 2011;49(3):339-359.

14. Hawley PH, Little TD, Card NA. The myth of the alpha male: A new look at dominance-related beliefs and behaviors among adolescent males and females. International Journal of Behavioral Development. January 1, 2008 2008;32(1):76-88.

15. Woods S, Wolke D, Novicki S, Hall L. Emotion recognition abilities and empathy of victims of bullying. Child Abuse and Neglect. 2009;33(5):307-311.

16. Tippett N, Wolke D. Socioeconomic Status and Bullying: A Meta-Analysis. American Journal of Public Health. 2014:e1-e12.

17. Tippett N, Wolke D, Platt L. Ethnicity and bullying involvement in a national UK youth sample. Journal of Adolescence. 2013;36(4):639-649.

18. Elgar FJ, Craig W, Boyce W, Morgan A, Vella-Zarb R. Income Inequality and School Bullying: Multilevel Study of Adolescents in 37 Countries. The Journal of Adolescent Health: Official publication of the Society for Adolescent Medicine. 10/01 2009;45(4):351-359.

19. Garandeau C, Lee I, Salmivalli C. Inequality Matters: Classroom Status Hierarchy and Adolescents’ Bullying. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2013/10/16 2013:1-11.

20. Wolke D, Skew AJ. Bullying among siblings. International Journal of Adolescent Medicine and Health. 2012;24(1):17-25.

21. Garandeau CF, Lee IA, Salmivalli C. Differential effects of the KiVa anti-bullying program on popular and unpopular bullies. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 2014;35(1):44-50.

22. Camodeca M, Goossens FA, Schuengel C, Terwogt MM. Links between social informative processing in middle childhood and involvement in bullying. Aggressive Behavior. 2003;29(2):116-127.

23. Wolke D, Skew A. Family factors, bullying victimisation and well-being in adolescents. Longitudinal and Life Course Studies. 2012;3(1):101-119.

24. Bogart LM, Elliott MN, Klein DJ, et al. Peer victimization in fifth grade and health in tenth grade. Pediatrics. February 17, 2014.

25. Zwierzynska K, Wolke D, Lereya TS. Peer victimization in childhood and internalizing problems in adolescence: a prospective longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2013/02/01 2013;41(2):309-323.

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

26. Lereya ST, Winsper C, Heron J, et al. Being bullied during childhood and the prospective pathways to self-harm in late adolescence. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2013;52(6):608-618.e602.

27. Sourander A, Jensen P, Ronning JA, et al. What is the early adulthood outcome of boys who bully or are bullied in childhood? The Finnish “From a Boy to a Man” study. Pediatrics. August 1, 2007;120(2):397-404.

28. Klomek AB, Sourander A, Niemelä S, et al. Childhood bullying behaviors as a risk for suicide attempts and completed suicides: A population-based birth cohort study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2009;48(3):254-261.

29. Takizawa R, Maughan B, Arseneault L. Adult Health outcomes of childhood bullying victimization: Evidence from a five-decade longitudinal british birth cohort. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2014:online first.

30. Wolke D, Schreier A, Zanarini MC, Winsper C. Bullied by peers in childhood and borderline personality symptoms at 11 years of age: A prospective study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2012;53(8):846-855.

31. Arseneault L, Milne BJ, Taylor A, et al. Being bullied as an environmentally mediated contributing factor to children’s internalizing problems: A study of twins discordant for victimization. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. February 1, 2008 2008;162(2):145-150.

32. Wolke D, Copeland WE, Angold A, Costello EJ. Impact of bullying in childhood on adult health, wealth, crime, and social outcomes. Psychological Science. August 19, 2013 2013;24(10):1958-1970.

33. Arseneault L, Bowes L, Shakoor S. Bullying victimization in youths and mental health problems: “Much ado about nothing”? Psychological Medicine. 2010;40(5):717-729.

34. Brown S, Taylor K. Bullying, education and earnings: Evidence from the National Child Development Study. Economics of Education Review. 2008;27(4):387-401.

35. Lehti V, Klomek AB, Tamminen T, et al. Childhood bullying and becoming a young father in a national cohort of Finnish boys. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology. 2012;53(6):461-466.

36. Sutton J, Smith PK, Swettenham J. Social cognition and bullying: Social inadequacy or skilled manipulation? British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 1999;17:435-450.

37. Ouellet-Morin I, Danese A, Bowes L, et al. A discordant monozygotic twin design shows blunted cortisol reactivity among bullied children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2011;50(6):574-582.e573.

38. Sugden K, Arseneault L, Harrington H, Moffitt TE, Williams B, Caspi A. Serotonin transporter gene moderates the development of emotional problems among children following bullying victimization. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2010;49(8):830-840.

39. Shalev I, Moffitt TE, Sugden K, et al. Exposure to violence during childhood is associated with telomere erosion from 5 to 10 years of age: A longitudinal study. Mol Psychiatry. 2012;18:576-581.

40. Harkness KL, Stewart JG, Wynne-Edwards KE. Cortisol reactivity to social stress in adolescents: Role of depression severity and child maltreatment. Psychoneuroendocrino. 2011;36(2):173-181.

41. Segerstrom SC, Miller GE. Psychological Stress and the human immune system: A meta-analytic study of 30 years of inquiry. Psychological Bulletin. 2004;30(4):601-630.

42. Copeland WE, Wolke D, Lereya ST, Shanahan L, Worthman C, Costello EJ. Childhood bullying involvement predicts low-grade systemic inflammation into adulthood. 2014. PNAS: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 111(21):7570-7575.

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

43. Jousilahti P, Salomaa V, Rasi V, Vahtera E, Palosuo T. Association of markers of systemic inflammation, C-reactive protein, serum amyloid A, and fibrinogen, with socioeconomic status. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. September 1, 2003 2003;57(9):730-733.

44. Sapolsky RM. The influence of social hierarchy on primate health. Science. April 29, 2005 2005;308(5722):648-652.

45. Mezulis AH, Abramson LY, Hyde JS, Hankin BL. Is there a universal positivity bias in attributions? A meta-analytic review of individual, developmental, and cultural differences in the self-serving attributional bias. Psychological Bulletin. 2004;130(5):711-747.

46. van Dam DS, van der Ven E, Velthorst E, Selten JP, Morgan C, de Haan L. Childhood bullying and the association with psychosis in non-clinical and clinical samples: A review and meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine. 2012;42(12):2463-2474.

47. Teicher MH, Samson JA, Sheu YS, Polcari A, McGreenery CE. Hurtful words: Association of exposure to peer verbal abuse with elevated psychiatric symptom scores and corpus callosum abnormalities. Am J Psychiat. 2010;167(12):1464-1471.

48. Nakamoto J, Schwartz D. Is peer victimization associated with academic achievement? A meta-analytic review. Social Development. 2010;19(2):221-242.

49. Brown V, Clery E, Ferguson C. Estimating the prevalence of young people absent from school due to bullying. National Centre for Social Research. 2011;1:1-61.

50. Masiello M, Schroeder D, Barto S, et al. 2012. The cost benefit: A first-time analysis of savings. Highmark Foundation.

51. Chamberlain T, George N, Golden S, Walker F, Benton T. 2010. Tellus4 national report: National Foundation for Educational Research. The Department for Children, Schools, and Families.

52. Radford L, Corral S, Bradley C, Fisher HL. 2013. The prevalence and impact of child maltreatment and other types of victimization in the UK: Findings from a population survey of caregivers, children and young people and young adults. Child Abuse and Neglect. 37(10):801-813.

53. Scrabstein JC, Merrick J. 2012. Bullying is everywhere: An expanding scope of public health concerns. International Journal of Adolescent Medicine and Health. 24(1):1.

54. Dale J, Russell R, Wolke D. 2014. Intervening in primary care against childhood bullying: An increasingly pressing public health need. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine. 107(6):219-223.

55. Sapouna M, Wolke D, Vannini N, et al. 2010. Virtual learning intervention to reduce bullying victimization in primary school: A controlled trial. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 51(1):104-112.

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×

This page intentionally left blank.

Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 75
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 76
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 77
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 78
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 79
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 80
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 81
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 82
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 83
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 84
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 85
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 86
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 87
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 88
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 89
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 90
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 91
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 92
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 93
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 94
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 95
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 96
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 97
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 98
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 99
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 100
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 101
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 102
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 103
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 104
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 105
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 106
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 107
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 108
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 109
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 110
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 111
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 112
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 113
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 114
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 115
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 116
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 117
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 118
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 119
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 120
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 121
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 122
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 123
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 124
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 125
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 126
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 127
Suggested Citation:"Appendix A: Workshop-Related Discussion Papers." National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2018. Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/24916.
×
Page 128
Next: Appendix B: Workshop Agenda »
Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop Get This Book
×
 Violence and Mental Health: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Detection: Proceedings of a Workshop
Buy Paperback | $50.00 Buy Ebook | $40.99
MyNAP members save 10% online.
Login or Register to save!
Download Free PDF

On February 26–27, 2014, the National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine’s Forum on Global Violence Prevention convened a workshop titled Mental Health and Violence: Opportunities for Prevention and Early Intervention. The workshop brought together advocates and experts in public health and mental health, anthropology, biomedical science, criminal justice, global health and development, and neuroscience to examine experience, evidence, and practice at the intersection of mental health and violence. Participants explored how violence impacts mental health and how mental health influences violence and discussed approaches to improve research and practice in both domains. This publication summarizes the presentations and discussions from the workshop.

READ FREE ONLINE

  1. ×

    Welcome to OpenBook!

    You're looking at OpenBook, NAP.edu's online reading room since 1999. Based on feedback from you, our users, we've made some improvements that make it easier than ever to read thousands of publications on our website.

    Do you want to take a quick tour of the OpenBook's features?

    No Thanks Take a Tour »
  2. ×

    Show this book's table of contents, where you can jump to any chapter by name.

    « Back Next »
  3. ×

    ...or use these buttons to go back to the previous chapter or skip to the next one.

    « Back Next »
  4. ×

    Jump up to the previous page or down to the next one. Also, you can type in a page number and press Enter to go directly to that page in the book.

    « Back Next »
  5. ×

    Switch between the Original Pages, where you can read the report as it appeared in print, and Text Pages for the web version, where you can highlight and search the text.

    « Back Next »
  6. ×

    To search the entire text of this book, type in your search term here and press Enter.

    « Back Next »
  7. ×

    Share a link to this book page on your preferred social network or via email.

    « Back Next »
  8. ×

    View our suggested citation for this chapter.

    « Back Next »
  9. ×

    Ready to take your reading offline? Click here to buy this book in print or download it as a free PDF, if available.

    « Back Next »
Stay Connected!