Skip to main content

Currently Skimming:

3 Workload Factors
Pages 54-93

The Chapter Skim interface presents what we've algorithmically identified as the most significant single chunk of text within every page in the chapter.
Select key terms on the right to highlight them within pages of the chapter.


From page 54...
... imposed task demands if the difficulty, number, rate, or complexity of the demands imposed on an operator are increased, workload is assumed to increase; (2) the level of performance an operator is able to achieve if errors increase or control precision degrades, workload is assumed to increase; (3)
From page 55...
... As task difficulty increases, performance often, but not always, degrades; response times and errors increase for discrete tasks, control variability and error increase for tracking tasks, and fewer tasks are completed within an interval of time. The workload imposed by one task may interfere with the performance of other concurrent activities.
From page 56...
... suggested that the relationship between workload and performance changes as overall task difficulty is increased. For relatively easy tasks, operators can maintain consistent performance by exerting additional effort if task demands are increased.
From page 57...
... Given the limitations of human memory, vision, physical strength, and so forth, some tasks may stretch or even exceed an operator's capacities; other tasks impose so few demands that they may be performed concurrently with other tasks. Thousands of experiments have been conducted in which a variety of task-difficulty manipulations were imposed to examine different aspects of the human information processing system and to define and quantify the limits of human capabilities.
From page 58...
... Thus, although minimizing error is the goal of most control activities, smoothness and stability may be equally important. Similarly, in discrete control tasks like target acquisition, performance strategies can be described by a tradeoff between the speed and accuracy of the movements.
From page 59...
... A similar relationship has been found between the movement difficulty and subjective workload (Hart et al., 1984b; Johnson and Hart, 1987; Mosier and Hart, 1986~. Instructions to maximize either speed or accuracy influence the workload and performance associated with discrete responses, target acquisition, and continuous control.
From page 60...
... . An operator's ability to respond to changing priorities is influenced by the total demands of concurrent tasks, their resource requirements, instructions, feedback, and training (Tsang and Wickens, 19881.
From page 61...
... Subjects also alternated between the control task and discrete tasks that took several seconds to complete. Response times increased significantly over single-task levels, and subjective workload was high, possibly reflecting the added cost of switching back and forth.
From page 62...
... , thus completing twice as many tasks in the same interval of time as low-scoring subjects, who were more reactive and performed task elements sequentially, even though there was sufficient time to alternate between tasks. In operational situations, team members may request assistance from each other as a strategy for coping with excessive task demands.
From page 63...
... In continuous control tasks, such as that performed by the tank driver, the operator's goal is to minimize the time-averaged difference between a target (e.g., a nominal flight path, a driving lane, a point or line on a screen) and the output of a dynamic system.
From page 64...
... As presentation rate was increased from once every 2.4 minutes to once every 0.8 minutes, subjective workload increased significantly. For complex tasks, the pattern of performance decrements that occurs when there is insufficient time to monitor, process, and respond to all task components depends on the strategy an operator adopts.
From page 65...
... found that map complexity had a more powerful effect on the cognitive operations required for navigation than any other variable; each additional landmark added to the map increased response time by 450 milliseconds. Variability of Task Demands People develop expectations about how much effort a specific task should require.
From page 66...
... For example, Thornton (1985) demonstrated that an unexpected peak of workload introduced in a relatively low workload task, elevated subjective workload for the whole segment, particularly if it occurred late in the interval.
From page 67...
... Even though takeoff and landing segments last only a few minutes and cruise segments last considerably longer, pilot workload ratings reflect the difficulty rather than the duration of a segment (Bortolussi et al., 1987; Hart and Hauser, 1987~. Pepitone et al.
From page 68...
... found that overall workload and subjective fatigue were unrelated to the duration of flights performed at night in the Kuiper Airborne Observatory of the National Aeronautics and Space Administration. Task Requirements and Procedures i It should be clear from the preceding discussion that the introduction of automation must be preceded by a careful analysis of task requirements and human capabilities.
From page 69...
... There is no question that crew workload is influenced by the number and complexity of the information sources that must be monitored and the quality of the information they provide. However, the influence of purely perceptual factors on workload has been examined in less depth than have information processing, memory, and response mechanisms.
From page 70...
... Interface design affects both workload and the cognitive strategies adopted (Woods, 1989~. For example, the design of display elements and their organization can force even a highly skilled user from automatic perceptual processes to effortful cognitive processes, or vice versa.
From page 71...
... Information From the Visual Scene In the real world, operators often rely on visual cues in the external scene (i.e., optical flow, structural transformations, terrain features) to esti mate their orientation, speed, altitude, heading, and location.
From page 72...
... Thus, objects may look very different than they do with the naked eye (Foyle et al., 19901; surface texture and shading are limited or inconsistent with the viewer's expectations; shadows, convex, and concave areas may be either bright or dark depending on the polarity selected; and objects may disappear if their temperature is similar to that of their surround. Again, the perceptual demands thus imposed add significantly to operator workload.
From page 73...
... Because the visual and auditory demands imposed on the operators of many complex systems are so high, and because incoming information may
From page 74...
... Instead, the focus has been on the processes required to interpret, transform, and remember information and formulate a response. Information Processing Variables As the cognitive processing required to interpret incoming information and select a response increases from simple rules of thumb to complex algorithms, inference, or deduction, response time generally increases; errors are more frequent; and experienced workload is higher.
From page 75...
... In fact, subjective workload estimates were often dominated by the difficulty of the skill-based component of the task. Since there is less uncertainty associated with skill- and rule-based behaviors, these tasks are often candidates for automation.
From page 76...
... conducted two simulations of advanced Army scout/attack helicopter missions to evaluate alternative methods of entering discrete commands. Although commands took longer to enter using a voice recognition system than with manual switches (and subjective workload was higher)
From page 77...
... Although humans appear to be aware of the total demands imposed on them (created by variations in task frequency, difficulty, and number) , their estimates of workload are relatively insensitive to resource competition.
From page 78...
... Since memory is fallible, particularly when interruptions occur, written procedures and checklists are used in many operational environments to reduce operator workload and decrease the probability of errors. However, since retrieving the appropriate page from a manual may take an unacceptable amount of time in an emergency situation, critical items (e.g., the first few steps in an emergency checklist)
From page 79...
... (1989) found that response time and subjective workload increased when mental rotation was required to bring two items into alignment for comparison.
From page 80...
... Poorly designed controls, high-order system dynamics, inadequate displays, and incompatible controls and displays may make it difficult for an operator to accomplish even relatively easy tasks. Control Design Many real-world tasks require discrete, transient, periodic, or relatively continuous control inputs in response to direct or indirect feedback from the environment.
From page 81...
... While high-gain systems reduce the workload of the initial phase of target acquisitions (the higher the gain, the smaller the control displacement required) , they impose additional workload during the final phase (the higher the gain, the finer the corrections required to remain over the target area)
From page 82...
... Put very simplistically, position, velocity, and acceleration controls require a minimum of one, two, or three control inputs to bring and hold a reticle over a target. Computer Aiding and Automation Computer aiding and automation are introduced to reduce operator workload, increase safety, improve performance, and extend mission capability.
From page 83...
... Because predictor displays perform some integration and projection for operators, thus allowing them to be proactive rather than reactive, they often improve the accuracy and smoothness of control activities and reduce operator workload. Stability augmentation systems reduce the frequency and order of control inputs required of an operator.
From page 84...
... Often, it simply replaces one form of workload (the physical demands of manual control) with another (the perceptual-cognitive demands of monitoring the system)
From page 85...
... Furthermore, incomplete attention to the current situation and tasks is more likely to result in errors, which will impose additional workload to rectify.
From page 86...
... Pp. 397-416 in Proceedings of the 20th Annual Conference on Manual Control.
From page 87...
... Pp. 100-115 in Proceedings of the 19th Annual Conference on Manual Control.
From page 88...
... Pp. 431-454 in Proceedings of the 20th Annual Conference on Manual Control, Volume 1.
From page 89...
... Pp. 127-149 in Proceedings of the 18th Annual Conference on Manual Control.
From page 90...
... Junker 1979 Effects of simulator delays on performance and learning in a roll-axis tracking task. Proceedings of the Fifteenth Annual Conference on Manual Control.
From page 91...
... Hart, and Y.Y. Yeh 1986 Memory and subjective workload assessment.
From page 92...
... M.A., and C.D. Wickens 1984 Subjective workload assessment and voluntary control of effort in a tracking task.
From page 93...
... Santa Monica, California: Hu man Factors Society. Wiener, E.L., and R.E.


This material may be derived from roughly machine-read images, and so is provided only to facilitate research.
More information on Chapter Skim is available.